Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Cognitive Processing 1/2022

09.11.2021 | Review

The implicit sense of agency is not a perceptual effect but is a judgment effect

verfasst von: Nagireddy Neelakanteswar Reddy

Erschienen in: Cognitive Processing | Ausgabe 1/2022

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

The sense of agency (SoA) is characterized as the sense of being the causal agent of one's own actions, and it is measured in two forms: explicit and implicit. In the explicit SoA experiments, the participants explicitly report whether they have a sense of control over their actions or whether they or somebody else is the causal agent of seen actions; the implicit SoA experiments study how do participants' agentive or voluntary actions modify perceptual processes (like time, vision, tactility, and audition) without directly asking the participants to explicitly think about their causal agency or sense of control. However, recent implicit SoA literature reported contradictory findings of the relationship between implicit SoA reports and agency states. Thus, I argue that the purported implicit SoA reports are not agency-driven perceptual effects per se but are judgment effects, by showing that (a) the typical operationalizations in implicit SoA domain lead to perceptual uncertainty on the part of the participants, (b) under uncertainty, participants' implicit SoA reports are due to heuristic judgments which are independent of agency states, and (c) under perceptual certainty, the typical implicit SoA reports might not have occurred at all. Thus, I conclude that the instances of implicit SoA are judgments (or response biases)—under uncertainty—rather than perceptual effects.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 390 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe




 

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Fußnoten
1
Although the implicit SoA is theoretically characterized to be implicit or pre-reflective, it is measured, in practice, as the explicit, conscious (temporal binding and sensory attenuation) reports. (However, in the visual perception domain, the term “implicit” implies an unconscious perception, which is indirectly deduced from the successful action in the absence of the conscious perception).
 
2
Some experiments (e.g., Hagura et al. 2012; Dewey and Carr 2013) found that voluntary (or agentive) action is perceived to be slower than the passive action (implying dilation rather than binding).
 
3
The Psychology literature makes a distinction between two usages of the term “judgment”: (1) any conscious and explicit verbal report (contra unconscious behavioral signature) that the participant makes in response to an experimental stimulus (e.g., Weber 1937), and (2) participants’ verbal report that is due to conceptual or inferential factors–as well as due to response biases or decisional factors–(contra verbal report that is based on genuine sensory perception) [e.g., Firestone and Scholl 2016]. In this paper, the term “judgment” refers to the second meaning i.e., a verbal report (particularly about perception) that is due to conceptual or cognitive factors rather than sensory (perceptual) factors.
 
4
The very fact that these inconsistent thoughts are judged to be contradictory implies that the person judging them is entertaining those thoughts; actually, it is due to entertaining those very contradictory thoughts that one can notice them to be contradictory in the first place. So, it is a different issue that these thoughts are contradictory and that these thoughts are not physically materializable, and it is a different issue that we can have contradictory thoughts and that these contradictory thoughts lead to epistemic discomfort and cognitive dissonance.
 
5
Benjamin Libet originally used this analog clock to study free will (Libet, Gleason, Wright, & Pearl, 1983) while Haggard et al., (2002) used it to study the implicit SoA. [Benjamin Libet found that the moment of intention is temporally later to the moment of brain activation (or the readiness potential) and argued that we do not have free will as the intention might not be the cause if it is not temporally prior – as by definition, the cause is temporally prior to the effect].
 
6
Haggard et al., (2002)’s first-ever task used TMS induced finger twitch as the passive condition.
 
7
In the typical intentional binding task using the Libet clock (Haggard et al., 2002) it is found that during the voluntary condition, the subjective time of action (execution) is reported (by the participants) to be later than the objective time of action execution and the subjective time of seeing the action-effect is reported to be earlier than the objective occurrence-time of action-effect. Whereas in the involuntary or passive condition, these effects happen in reverse, i.e., the subjective time of action (execution) is reported (by the participants) to be earlier than the objective time of action (execution) and the subjective time of seeing the action-effect is reported to be later than the objective occurrence-time of action-effect.
 
8
In the verbal estimation tasks, the participants undergo exposure to the temporal interval, and they have to report the experienced interval or duration in chronometric units such as milliseconds, seconds or minutes (however, the verbal estimation method faces the problem that the participants tend to round off the time duration). In the time production task, the experimenter specifies the duration/interval in the chronometric units, and the participant produces the specified duration, e.g., by pressing a button for that specified amount of time. In the time reproduction task, the participant is presented with a sample or target duration (unlike the experimenter’s specification of time in chronometric units), e.g., as a continuous flash or sound and the participant has to reproduce the same duration, e.g., by pressing the button for the same amount of time equivalent to that of the previously presented sample duration. In the time comparison or the time discrimination task, the participant is being presented with two sequential intervals or durations, e.g., as continuous sounds or flashes, and the participant has to compare and differentiate between these two time intervals with regard to their relative time frame, i.e., whether the first one is shorter or longer than the second time interval—this is also referred to as the temporal bisection task.
 
9
Mathematically put, the interval of uncertainty’s range is 2 JNDs, or “the interval of uncertainty = upper limen – lower limen,” i.e., it ranges from the JND below the point of subjective equality to the JND above the point of subjective equality.
 
10
In the Libet clock based IB task, not only an extrapolation at the initial position of the clock’s dot/ hand occurs, but there also occurs a retropolation of the dot/hand at the final position of its motion.
 
11
But, Getzmann and Lewald (2007) found that motion extrapolation occurs at the initial and final positions of the moving object, and motion retropolation occurs in the middle phases. Hubbard and Motes (2002), Hubbard and Ruppel (2011) also found that motion extrapolation occurs at the final position, and retropolation at the initial position. So, the extrapolation or the retropolation, at either initial or final position, of the moving object are not consistent findings.
 
12
Some researchers attribute motion mislocalization effects to attentional factors (e.g., Hubbard, Kumar, & Carp, 2009; Hubbard & Ruppel, 2011). However, attentional explanation still discredits the perceptual explanation, as attention influences occur at the level of input rather than the level of (central) processing of the perceptual system (Firestone & Scholl, 2016; Raftopoulos, 2017).
 
13
Petzschner et al. (2015) put that the regression effect is the “tendency of subjective estimates to be biased towards the center of the distribution” (p. 286), range effect is “an increase of this bias for larger sample ranges” (p. 286), scalar variability is “a linear increase in standard deviation of estimates with mean magnitude” (p. 286), sequential or order effects are the “correlations between subsequent magnitude judgments” (p. 286).
 
14
The constant error is “a stimulus value equal to the value of the point of subjective equality minus the value of the standard stimulus” (Gescheider, 1997, p. 394).
 
15
It has to be noted that the magnitude estimation value is the mean (of all those wavering individual trials’ estimations) of an experimental block. These consistent but inaccurate magnitude estimation biases such as the constant error, regression effect, range effect, and sequential or order effects (that are ultimately calculated as the mean) can just be due to the skewing of those wavering estimations rather than due to a genuine sensory effect, as the participants can freely vary their estimations in the range/interval of uncertainty.
 
16
Motion extrapolative mislocalization happens over the action-effects of others (Hudson et al. 2016; Hudson et al. 2018) and over passive movements (Nijhawan and Khurana 2003) too.
 
17
Dissociation between different cognitive processes is anticipated by the “modularity of mind” hypothesis.
 
18
Correlation does not imply causation, but non-correlation implies non-causation (assuming that the relationship in test is linear in nature).
 
19
But, see Piras and Coull (2011) for the claim of a common mechanism behind implicit action and explicit duration reports.
 
20
Motion extrapolation is reduced when the motion is towards left, compared to when it is to towards right (Halpern & Kelly, 1993) implying that extrapolation is based on the learning or prior knowledge or belief that the motion happens towards the right side.
 
21
But, the motion mislocalizations are also attributed to compensation for neural delays (Eagleman & Sejnowski, 2007; Khoei et al. 2017; Nijhawan, 2008), perceptual latency (Krekelberg and Lappe 2001; Patel, Ogmen, Bedell, & Sampath, 2000), and crossmodal influences (Hubbard & Courtney, 2010) etc., (see, Hubbard, 2017; Maus, Khurana, & Nijhawan, 2010 for reviews).
 
22
Some researchers (e.g., Courtney & Hubbard, 2008) interpret these effects to be instances of cognitive penetration in motion perception, but I stress on the possibility of judgment effects under the inherent uncertainty in speedy motion perception.
 
23
Further, a general theory, "A theory of magnitude" (AToM) [Walsh, 2003] makes a link between magnitude estimation and attribute-substitution by proposing that humans have a tendency to substitute one magnitude with that of the other, across the domains of numbers, time, and space.
 
24
There is also evidence that the IB bias vanishes if motion localization in the Libet clock is performed by mouse click rather than as a verbal report (Joordens, Spalek, Razmy, & Van Duijn, 2004).
 
25
The intensity attenuation operationalization is incomplete as a test of perception, as we do not exclusively perceive intensities of the stimuli such as low or high auditory loudness or visual brightness etc., but we do also perceive categories such as (auditory categories like) bells and thunders or (visual categories like) faces and houses. Although there is a prevalence of metric/magnitudinal perceptual stimuli (in the implicit SoA operationalizations) the categorical percepts are equally relevant for SoA operationalization as we do “act” or “agentive upon” on categories such as bells, houses, faces also.
 
26
Is it spurious to distinguish between categories and magnitudes with respect to psychophysical judgments? No! For instance, stimuli of different categories, e.g., bell sound and laughter sound with the same intensity or magnitude will still be perceived differently; so the distinction between magnitudes and categories is not trivial.
 
27
But see, Cardoso-Leite et al., (2010) for the presence of sensory attenuation using stimuli of Gabor patches. However, this finding is due to the fact that the perception of Gabor patches can be converted into a magnitude estimation task if the experimental question used is “how much tilted is the orientation of Gabor patch?”.
 
28
However, this sort of categorical questioning is rarely operationalized in the implicit SoA experiments. If one uses the category kind of question, then there would not be any evidence of agency’s influence as the participants can always distinguish the big and small intensities (as long as they fall beyond the range of interval of uncertainty).
 
29
According to the power law “the relationship between sensation magnitude and stimulus intensity is a power function in which sensation magnitude is proportional to stimulus intensity raised to a power” (Gescheider, 1997, p. 402).
 
30
Similarly, concerning time perception, the question of “how long is it” (rather than “is it short or long” question) is typically asked in time perception tasks such as verbal estimation of duration, time interval production, time interval reproduction. However, the duration perception, which is measured as a metric/magnitudinal type (through the measures of duration estimation, temporal production, etc.), can also be made categorical by changing the question from "how long" to "is it short or long (among two different durations)" by employing the temporal bisection task (Penney and Cheng 2018) as the temporal bisection too obeys power-law or the power function proportionality (Kopec and Brody 2010) which I contend that nullifies the top-down influences (from agency states) in time perception.
 
31
Although real-life demonstrations have a bane of uncontrollability from extraneous variables, they have a boon of ecological validity.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Acerbi L, Wolpert DM, Vijayakumar S (2012) Internal representations of temporal statistics and feedback calibrate motor-sensory interval timing. PLoS Comput Biol 8(11):e1002771PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Acerbi L, Wolpert DM, Vijayakumar S (2012) Internal representations of temporal statistics and feedback calibrate motor-sensory interval timing. PLoS Comput Biol 8(11):e1002771PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Antusch S, Custers R, Marien H, Aarts H (2020) Intentionality and temporal binding: do causality beliefs increase the perceived temporal attraction between events? Conscious Cogn 77:102835PubMedCrossRef Antusch S, Custers R, Marien H, Aarts H (2020) Intentionality and temporal binding: do causality beliefs increase the perceived temporal attraction between events? Conscious Cogn 77:102835PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Aschersleben G, Müsseler J (1999) Dissociations in the timing of stationary and moving stimuli. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25(6):1709CrossRef Aschersleben G, Müsseler J (1999) Dissociations in the timing of stationary and moving stimuli. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25(6):1709CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Aschersleben, G. (1999). Task-dependent timing of perceptual events. In Advances in Psychology (Vol. 129, pp. 293–318). North-Holland. Aschersleben, G. (1999). Task-dependent timing of perceptual events. In Advances in Psychology (Vol. 129, pp. 293–318). North-Holland.
Zurück zum Zitat Ashida H (2004) Action-specific extrapolation of target motion in human visual system. Neuropsychologia 42(11):1515–1524PubMedCrossRef Ashida H (2004) Action-specific extrapolation of target motion in human visual system. Neuropsychologia 42(11):1515–1524PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Banks W, Isham E (2010) Do we really know what we are doing? Implications of reported time of decision for theories of volition. In: Sinnott-Armstrong W, Nadel L (eds) Conscious will and responsibility: A tribute to Benjamin Libet. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 47–60CrossRef Banks W, Isham E (2010) Do we really know what we are doing? Implications of reported time of decision for theories of volition. In: Sinnott-Armstrong W, Nadel L (eds) Conscious will and responsibility: A tribute to Benjamin Libet. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 47–60CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bayne T (2008) The phenomenology of agency. Philos Compass 3(1):182–202CrossRef Bayne T (2008) The phenomenology of agency. Philos Compass 3(1):182–202CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bermúdez JL (2007) What is at stake in the debate on nonconceptual content? Philos Perspect 21(1):55–72CrossRef Bermúdez JL (2007) What is at stake in the debate on nonconceptual content? Philos Perspect 21(1):55–72CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Blakemore SJ, Wolpert D, Frith C (2000) Why can’t you tickle yourself? NeuroReport 11(11):R11–R16PubMedCrossRef Blakemore SJ, Wolpert D, Frith C (2000) Why can’t you tickle yourself? NeuroReport 11(11):R11–R16PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Blättler C, Ferrari V, Didierjean A, Van Elslande P, Marmèche E (2010) Can expertise modulate representational momentum? Vis Cogn 18(9):1253–1273CrossRef Blättler C, Ferrari V, Didierjean A, Van Elslande P, Marmèche E (2010) Can expertise modulate representational momentum? Vis Cogn 18(9):1253–1273CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Block N (2014) Seeing-as in the light of vision science. Philos Phenomenol Res 89(1):560–572CrossRef Block N (2014) Seeing-as in the light of vision science. Philos Phenomenol Res 89(1):560–572CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brenner E, Smeets JB (2010) How well can people judge when something happened? Vision Res 50(12):1101–1108PubMedCrossRef Brenner E, Smeets JB (2010) How well can people judge when something happened? Vision Res 50(12):1101–1108PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brenner E, van Beers RJ, Rotman G, Smeets JB (2006) The role of uncertainty in the systematic spatial mislocalization of moving objects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(4):811PubMedCrossRef Brenner E, van Beers RJ, Rotman G, Smeets JB (2006) The role of uncertainty in the systematic spatial mislocalization of moving objects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(4):811PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brocas I, Carrillo JD, Tarrasó J (2018) Self-awareness of biases in time perception. J Econ Behav Organ 148:1–19CrossRef Brocas I, Carrillo JD, Tarrasó J (2018) Self-awareness of biases in time perception. J Econ Behav Organ 148:1–19CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brouwer AM, Franz VH, Thornton IM (2004) Representational momentum in perception and grasping: translating versus transforming objects. J vis 4(7):5–5CrossRef Brouwer AM, Franz VH, Thornton IM (2004) Representational momentum in perception and grasping: translating versus transforming objects. J vis 4(7):5–5CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Buehner MJ (2010) Temporal binding. In: Nobre AC, Coull JT (eds) Attention and time. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 201–211CrossRef Buehner MJ (2010) Temporal binding. In: Nobre AC, Coull JT (eds) Attention and time. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 201–211CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Buehner MJ (2012) Understanding the past, predicting the future: causation, not intentional action, is the root of temporal binding. Psychol Sci 23(12):1490–1497PubMedCrossRef Buehner MJ (2012) Understanding the past, predicting the future: causation, not intentional action, is the root of temporal binding. Psychol Sci 23(12):1490–1497PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Buehner MJ (2015) Awareness of voluntary and involuntary causal actions and their outcomes. Psychol Conscious Theory Res Pract 2(3):237 Buehner MJ (2015) Awareness of voluntary and involuntary causal actions and their outcomes. Psychol Conscious Theory Res Pract 2(3):237
Zurück zum Zitat Buehner MJ, Humphreys GR (2009) Causal binding of actions to their effects. Psychol Sci 20(10):1221–1228PubMedCrossRef Buehner MJ, Humphreys GR (2009) Causal binding of actions to their effects. Psychol Sci 20(10):1221–1228PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bueti D, Walsh V (2010) Memory for time distinguishes between perception and action. Perception 39(1):81–90PubMedCrossRef Bueti D, Walsh V (2010) Memory for time distinguishes between perception and action. Perception 39(1):81–90PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Burge T (2010) Origins of Objectivity. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRef Burge T (2010) Origins of Objectivity. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Burin D, Pyasik M, Ronga I, Cavallo M, Salatino A, Pia L (2018) “As long as that is my hand, that willed action is mine”: timing of agency triggered by body ownership. Conscious Cogn 58:186–192PubMedCrossRef Burin D, Pyasik M, Ronga I, Cavallo M, Salatino A, Pia L (2018) “As long as that is my hand, that willed action is mine”: timing of agency triggered by body ownership. Conscious Cogn 58:186–192PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cardoso-Leite P, Mamassian P, Schütz-Bosbach S, Waszak F (2010) A new look at sensory attenuation: action-effect anticipation affects sensitivity, not response bias. Psychol Sci 21(12):1740–1745PubMedCrossRef Cardoso-Leite P, Mamassian P, Schütz-Bosbach S, Waszak F (2010) A new look at sensory attenuation: action-effect anticipation affects sensitivity, not response bias. Psychol Sci 21(12):1740–1745PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Carey S (2009) The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRef Carey S (2009) The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Carruthers G (2012) The case for the comparator model as an explanation of the sense of agency and its breakdowns. Conscious Cogn 21(1):30–45PubMedCrossRef Carruthers G (2012) The case for the comparator model as an explanation of the sense of agency and its breakdowns. Conscious Cogn 21(1):30–45PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Castiello U, Paulignan Y, Jeannerod M (1991) Temporal dissociation of motor responses and subjective awareness: a study in normal subjects. Brain 114(6):2639–2655PubMedCrossRef Castiello U, Paulignan Y, Jeannerod M (1991) Temporal dissociation of motor responses and subjective awareness: a study in normal subjects. Brain 114(6):2639–2655PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Corveleyn X, López-Moliner J, Coello Y (2012) Motor action reduces temporal asynchrony between perceived visual changes. J vis 12(11):20–20PubMedCrossRef Corveleyn X, López-Moliner J, Coello Y (2012) Motor action reduces temporal asynchrony between perceived visual changes. J vis 12(11):20–20PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Courtney JR, Hubbard TL (2008) Spatial memory and explicit knowledge: an effect of instruction on representational momentum. Q J Exp Psychol 61(12):1778–1784CrossRef Courtney JR, Hubbard TL (2008) Spatial memory and explicit knowledge: an effect of instruction on representational momentum. Q J Exp Psychol 61(12):1778–1784CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Crane T (1992) The nonconceptual content of experience. In: Crane T (ed) The Contents of Experience. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 136–157CrossRef Crane T (1992) The nonconceptual content of experience. In: Crane T (ed) The Contents of Experience. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 136–157CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cravo AM, Haddad H, Claessens PM, Baldo MV (2013) Bias and learning in temporal binding: intervals between actions and outcomes are compressed by prior bias. Conscious Cogn 22(4):1174–1180PubMedCrossRef Cravo AM, Haddad H, Claessens PM, Baldo MV (2013) Bias and learning in temporal binding: intervals between actions and outcomes are compressed by prior bias. Conscious Cogn 22(4):1174–1180PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Damen TG, Van Baaren RB, Brass M, Aarts H, Dijksterhuis A (2015) Put your plan into action: the influence of action plans on agency and responsibility. J Pers Soc Psychol 108(6):850PubMedCrossRef Damen TG, Van Baaren RB, Brass M, Aarts H, Dijksterhuis A (2015) Put your plan into action: the influence of action plans on agency and responsibility. J Pers Soc Psychol 108(6):850PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Danquah, A. N., Farrell, M. J., & O'Boyle, D. J. (2008). Biases in the subjective timing of perceptual events: Libet et al. (1983) revisited. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 616–627. Danquah, A. N., Farrell, M. J., & O'Boyle, D. J. (2008). Biases in the subjective timing of perceptual events: Libet et al. (1983) revisited. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 616–627.
Zurück zum Zitat Desantis A, Roussel C, Waszak F (2011) On the influence of causal beliefs on the feeling of agency. Conscious Cogn 20(4):1211–1220PubMedCrossRef Desantis A, Roussel C, Waszak F (2011) On the influence of causal beliefs on the feeling of agency. Conscious Cogn 20(4):1211–1220PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Desantis A, Hughes G, Waszak F (2012a) Intentional binding is driven by the mere presence of an action and not by motor prediction. PLoS ONE 7(1):e29557PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Desantis A, Hughes G, Waszak F (2012a) Intentional binding is driven by the mere presence of an action and not by motor prediction. PLoS ONE 7(1):e29557PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Desantis A, Weiss C, Schütz-Bosbach S, Waszak F (2012b) Believing and perceiving: authorship belief modulates sensory attenuation. PLoS ONE 7(5):e37959PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Desantis A, Weiss C, Schütz-Bosbach S, Waszak F (2012b) Believing and perceiving: authorship belief modulates sensory attenuation. PLoS ONE 7(5):e37959PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Desantis A, Waszak F, Moutsopoulou K, Haggard P (2016) How action structures time: about the perceived temporal order of action and predicted outcomes. Cognition 146:100–109PubMedCrossRef Desantis A, Waszak F, Moutsopoulou K, Haggard P (2016) How action structures time: about the perceived temporal order of action and predicted outcomes. Cognition 146:100–109PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dewey JA, Carr TH (2013) Predictable and self-initiated visual motion is judged to be slower than computer generated motion. Conscious Cogn 22(3):987–995PubMedCrossRef Dewey JA, Carr TH (2013) Predictable and self-initiated visual motion is judged to be slower than computer generated motion. Conscious Cogn 22(3):987–995PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Didierjean, A., Ferrari, V., & Blättler, C. (2014). Role of knowledge in motion extrapolation: the relevance of an approach contrasting experts and novices. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 61, pp. 215–235). Academic Press. Didierjean, A., Ferrari, V., & Blättler, C. (2014). Role of knowledge in motion extrapolation: the relevance of an approach contrasting experts and novices. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 61, pp. 215–235). Academic Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Dogge M, Schaap M, Custers R, Wegner DM, Aarts H (2012) When moving without volition: implied self-causation enhances binding strength between involuntary actions and effects. Conscious Cogn 21(1):501–506PubMedCrossRef Dogge M, Schaap M, Custers R, Wegner DM, Aarts H (2012) When moving without volition: implied self-causation enhances binding strength between involuntary actions and effects. Conscious Cogn 21(1):501–506PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dogge M, Hofman D, Custers R, Aarts H (2019) Exploring the role of motor and non-motor predictive mechanisms in sensory attenuation: perceptual and neurophysiological findings. Neuropsychologia 124:216–225PubMedCrossRef Dogge M, Hofman D, Custers R, Aarts H (2019) Exploring the role of motor and non-motor predictive mechanisms in sensory attenuation: perceptual and neurophysiological findings. Neuropsychologia 124:216–225PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dretske F (1981) Knowledge and the flow of information. The MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts Dretske F (1981) Knowledge and the flow of information. The MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts
Zurück zum Zitat Droit-Volet S, Lamotte M, Izaute M (2015) The conscious awareness of time distortions regulates the effect of emotion on the perception of time. Conscious Cogn 38:155–164PubMedCrossRef Droit-Volet S, Lamotte M, Izaute M (2015) The conscious awareness of time distortions regulates the effect of emotion on the perception of time. Conscious Cogn 38:155–164PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Eagleman DM, Sejnowski TJ (2007) Motion signals bias localization judgments: a unified explanation for the flash-lag, flash-drag, flash-jump, and Frohlich illusions. J vis 7(4):3–3PubMedCrossRef Eagleman DM, Sejnowski TJ (2007) Motion signals bias localization judgments: a unified explanation for the flash-lag, flash-drag, flash-jump, and Frohlich illusions. J vis 7(4):3–3PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Engbert K, Wohlschläger A, Thomas R, Haggard P (2007) Agency, subjective time, and other minds. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 33(6):1261PubMedCrossRef Engbert K, Wohlschläger A, Thomas R, Haggard P (2007) Agency, subjective time, and other minds. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 33(6):1261PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Engbert K, Wohlschläger A, Haggard P (2008) Who is causing what? The sense of agency is relational and efferent-triggered. Cognition 107(2):693–704PubMedCrossRef Engbert K, Wohlschläger A, Haggard P (2008) Who is causing what? The sense of agency is relational and efferent-triggered. Cognition 107(2):693–704PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Evans G (1982) The varieties of reference. Oxford University Press, Oxford Evans G (1982) The varieties of reference. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Zurück zum Zitat Faro D, McGill AL, Hastie R (2010) Naïve theories of causal force and compression of elapsed time judgments. J Pers Soc Psychol 98(5):683PubMedCrossRef Faro D, McGill AL, Hastie R (2010) Naïve theories of causal force and compression of elapsed time judgments. J Pers Soc Psychol 98(5):683PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Faro D, McGill AL, Hastie R (2013) The influence of perceived causation on judgments of time: an integrative review and implications for decision-making. Front Psychol 4:217PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Faro D, McGill AL, Hastie R (2013) The influence of perceived causation on judgments of time: an integrative review and implications for decision-making. Front Psychol 4:217PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Figdor, C. (2020). Experiences of duration and cognitive penetrability. The Epistemology of Non-Visual Perception, 188. Figdor, C. (2020). Experiences of duration and cognitive penetrability. The Epistemology of Non-Visual Perception, 188.
Zurück zum Zitat Firestone, C., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: evaluating the evidence for" top-down" effects. Behav Brain Sci, 39. Firestone, C., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: evaluating the evidence for" top-down" effects. Behav Brain Sci, 39.
Zurück zum Zitat Fodor JA (2008) LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford University Press, New YorkCrossRef Fodor JA (2008) LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford University Press, New YorkCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gallagher S (2000) Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci 4(1):14–21PubMedCrossRef Gallagher S (2000) Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci 4(1):14–21PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ganzenmüller S, Shi Z, Mueller HJ (2012) Duration reproduction with sensory feedback delay: differential involvement of perception and action time. Front Integr Neurosci 6:95PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ganzenmüller S, Shi Z, Mueller HJ (2012) Duration reproduction with sensory feedback delay: differential involvement of perception and action time. Front Integr Neurosci 6:95PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gescheider, G. A. (1997). Psychophysics: The Fundamentals. Psychology Press. Gescheider, G. A. (1997). Psychophysics: The Fundamentals. Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Getzmann S, Lewald J (2007) Localization of moving sound. Percept Psychophys 69(6):1022–1034PubMedCrossRef Getzmann S, Lewald J (2007) Localization of moving sound. Percept Psychophys 69(6):1022–1034PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grondin S (2008) Methods for studying psychological time. In: Grondin S (ed) Psychology of Time. Emerald, Bingley, UK, pp 51–74 Grondin S (2008) Methods for studying psychological time. In: Grondin S (ed) Psychology of Time. Emerald, Bingley, UK, pp 51–74
Zurück zum Zitat Haering C, Kiesel A (2012) Time in action contexts: learning when an action effect occurs. Psychol Res 76(3):336–344PubMedCrossRef Haering C, Kiesel A (2012) Time in action contexts: learning when an action effect occurs. Psychol Res 76(3):336–344PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Haering C, Kiesel A (2014) Intentional binding is independent of the validity of the action effect’s identity. Acta Physiol (oxf) 152:109–119 Haering C, Kiesel A (2014) Intentional binding is independent of the validity of the action effect’s identity. Acta Physiol (oxf) 152:109–119
Zurück zum Zitat Haggard P, Cole J (2007) Intention, attention and the temporal experience of action. Conscious Cogn 16(2):211–220PubMedCrossRef Haggard P, Cole J (2007) Intention, attention and the temporal experience of action. Conscious Cogn 16(2):211–220PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Haggard P, Clark S, Kalogeras J (2002) Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nat Neurosci 5(4):382PubMedCrossRef Haggard P, Clark S, Kalogeras J (2002) Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nat Neurosci 5(4):382PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hagura N, Kanai R, Orgs G, Haggard P (2012) Ready steady slow: action preparation slows the subjective passage of time. Proc Royal Soci B: Biol Sci 279(1746):4399–4406CrossRef Hagura N, Kanai R, Orgs G, Haggard P (2012) Ready steady slow: action preparation slows the subjective passage of time. Proc Royal Soci B: Biol Sci 279(1746):4399–4406CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Halpern AR, Kelly MH (1993) Memory biases in left versus right implied motion. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 19(2):471PubMedCrossRef Halpern AR, Kelly MH (1993) Memory biases in left versus right implied motion. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 19(2):471PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hass J, Blaschke S, Herrmann JM (2012) Cross-modal distortion of time perception: demerging the effects of observed and performed motion. PLoS ONE 7(6):e38092PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hass J, Blaschke S, Herrmann JM (2012) Cross-modal distortion of time perception: demerging the effects of observed and performed motion. PLoS ONE 7(6):e38092PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Heck RG (2007) Are there different kinds of content? In: Cohen J, McLaughlin B (eds) Contemporary Debates in the Philosophy of Mind. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 117–138 Heck RG (2007) Are there different kinds of content? In: Cohen J, McLaughlin B (eds) Contemporary Debates in the Philosophy of Mind. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 117–138
Zurück zum Zitat Herai T, Mogi K (2014) Perception of temporal duration affected by automatic and controlled movements. Conscious Cogn 29:23–35PubMedCrossRef Herai T, Mogi K (2014) Perception of temporal duration affected by automatic and controlled movements. Conscious Cogn 29:23–35PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard TL (1990) Cognitive representation of linear motion: possible direction and gravity effects in judged displacement. Mem Cognit 18(3):299–309PubMedCrossRef Hubbard TL (1990) Cognitive representation of linear motion: possible direction and gravity effects in judged displacement. Mem Cognit 18(3):299–309PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard TL (1995) Environmental invariants in the representation of motion: Implied dynamics and representational momentum, gravity, friction, and centripetal force. Psychon Bull Rev 2(3):322–338PubMedCrossRef Hubbard TL (1995) Environmental invariants in the representation of motion: Implied dynamics and representational momentum, gravity, friction, and centripetal force. Psychon Bull Rev 2(3):322–338PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard TL (2005) Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: a review of the findings. Psychon Bull Rev 12(5):822–851PubMedCrossRef Hubbard TL (2005) Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: a review of the findings. Psychon Bull Rev 12(5):822–851PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard TL (2017) Toward a general theory of momentum-like effects. Behav Proc 141:50–66CrossRef Hubbard TL (2017) Toward a general theory of momentum-like effects. Behav Proc 141:50–66CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard TL, Courtney JR (2008) The onset-repulsion effect and motion-induced mislocalization of a stationary object. Perception 37(9):1386–1398PubMedCrossRef Hubbard TL, Courtney JR (2008) The onset-repulsion effect and motion-induced mislocalization of a stationary object. Perception 37(9):1386–1398PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard TL, Courtney JR (2010) Cross-modal influences on representational momentum and representational gravity. Perception 39(6):851–862PubMedCrossRef Hubbard TL, Courtney JR (2010) Cross-modal influences on representational momentum and representational gravity. Perception 39(6):851–862PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard TL, Lange M (2010) Prior probabilities and representational momentum. Vis Cogn 18(7):1063–1087CrossRef Hubbard TL, Lange M (2010) Prior probabilities and representational momentum. Vis Cogn 18(7):1063–1087CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard TL, Motes MA (2002) Does representational momentum reflect a distortion of the length or the endpoint of a trajectory? Cognition 82(3):B89–B99PubMedCrossRef Hubbard TL, Motes MA (2002) Does representational momentum reflect a distortion of the length or the endpoint of a trajectory? Cognition 82(3):B89–B99PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard TL, Ruppel SE (2011) Effects of spatial cuing on the onset repulsion effect. Atten Percept Psychophys 73(7):2236PubMedCrossRef Hubbard TL, Ruppel SE (2011) Effects of spatial cuing on the onset repulsion effect. Atten Percept Psychophys 73(7):2236PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard TL, Kumar AM, Carp CL (2009) Effects of spatial cueing on representational momentum. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 35(3):666PubMedCrossRef Hubbard TL, Kumar AM, Carp CL (2009) Effects of spatial cueing on representational momentum. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 35(3):666PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hudson M, Nicholson T, Ellis R, Bach P (2016) I see what you say: prior knowledge of other’s goals automatically biases the perception of their actions. Cognition 146:245–250PubMedCrossRef Hudson M, Nicholson T, Ellis R, Bach P (2016) I see what you say: prior knowledge of other’s goals automatically biases the perception of their actions. Cognition 146:245–250PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hudson M, Bach P, Nicholson T (2018) You said you would! The predictability of other’s behavior from their intentions determines predictive biases in action perception. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 44(2):320PubMedCrossRef Hudson M, Bach P, Nicholson T (2018) You said you would! The predictability of other’s behavior from their intentions determines predictive biases in action perception. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 44(2):320PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hughes G, Desantis A, Waszak F (2013) Mechanisms of intentional binding and sensory attenuation: the role of temporal prediction, temporal control, identity prediction, and motor prediction. Psychol Bull 139(1):133PubMedCrossRef Hughes G, Desantis A, Waszak F (2013) Mechanisms of intentional binding and sensory attenuation: the role of temporal prediction, temporal control, identity prediction, and motor prediction. Psychol Bull 139(1):133PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Humphreys GR, Buehner MJ (2009) Magnitude estimation reveals temporal binding at super-second intervals. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 35(5):1542PubMedCrossRef Humphreys GR, Buehner MJ (2009) Magnitude estimation reveals temporal binding at super-second intervals. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 35(5):1542PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Humphreys GR, Buehner MJ (2010) Temporal binding of action and effect in interval reproduction. Exp Brain Res 203(2):465–470PubMedCrossRef Humphreys GR, Buehner MJ (2010) Temporal binding of action and effect in interval reproduction. Exp Brain Res 203(2):465–470PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ichikawa M, Masakura Y (2010) Reduction of the flash-lag effect in terms of active observation. Atten Percept Psychophys 72(4):1032–1044PubMedCrossRef Ichikawa M, Masakura Y (2010) Reduction of the flash-lag effect in terms of active observation. Atten Percept Psychophys 72(4):1032–1044PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Imaizumi S, Asai T (2017) My action lasts longer: potential link between subjective time and agency during voluntary action. Conscious Cogn 51:243–257PubMedCrossRef Imaizumi S, Asai T (2017) My action lasts longer: potential link between subjective time and agency during voluntary action. Conscious Cogn 51:243–257PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Iwasaki M, Tomita K, Noguchi Y (2017) Non-uniform transformation of subjective time during action preparation. Cognition 160:51–61PubMedCrossRef Iwasaki M, Tomita K, Noguchi Y (2017) Non-uniform transformation of subjective time during action preparation. Cognition 160:51–61PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jaśkowski, P. (1999). Reaction time and temporal-order judgment as measures of perceptual latency: the problem of dissociations. In Advances in Psychology (Vol. 129, pp. 265–282). North-Holland. Jaśkowski, P. (1999). Reaction time and temporal-order judgment as measures of perceptual latency: the problem of dissociations. In Advances in Psychology (Vol. 129, pp. 265–282). North-Holland.
Zurück zum Zitat Jensen, M., Di Costa, S., & Haggard, P. (2015). Intentional binding: A measure of agency. In Overgaard, M (Ed.,). Behavioral Methods in Consciousness Research (pp. 145–158). Oxford University Press. Jensen, M., Di Costa, S., & Haggard, P. (2015). Intentional binding: A measure of agency. In Overgaard, M (Ed.,). Behavioral Methods in Consciousness Research (pp. 145–158). Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Joordens S, Van Duijn M, Spalek TM (2002) When timing the mind one should also mind the timing: biases in the measurement of voluntary actions. Conscious Cogn 11:231–240PubMedCrossRef Joordens S, Van Duijn M, Spalek TM (2002) When timing the mind one should also mind the timing: biases in the measurement of voluntary actions. Conscious Cogn 11:231–240PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Joordens S, Spalek TM, Razmy S, Van Duijn M (2004) A clockwork orange: compensation opposing momentum in memory for location. Mem Cognit 32(1):39–50PubMedCrossRef Joordens S, Spalek TM, Razmy S, Van Duijn M (2004) A clockwork orange: compensation opposing momentum in memory for location. Mem Cognit 32(1):39–50PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jordan JS, Hunsinger M (2008) Learned patterns of action-effect anticipation contribute to the spatial displacement of continuously moving stimuli. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 34(1):113PubMedCrossRef Jordan JS, Hunsinger M (2008) Learned patterns of action-effect anticipation contribute to the spatial displacement of continuously moving stimuli. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 34(1):113PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kaiser J, Schütz-Bosbach S (2018) Sensory attenuation of self-produced signals does not rely on self-specific motor predictions. Eur J Neurosci 47(11):1303–1310PubMedCrossRef Kaiser J, Schütz-Bosbach S (2018) Sensory attenuation of self-produced signals does not rely on self-specific motor predictions. Eur J Neurosci 47(11):1303–1310PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kanai R, Sheth BR, Shimojo S (2004) Stopping the motion and sleuthing the flash-lag effect: spatial uncertainty is the key to perceptual mislocalization. Vision Res 44(22):2605–2619PubMedCrossRef Kanai R, Sheth BR, Shimojo S (2004) Stopping the motion and sleuthing the flash-lag effect: spatial uncertainty is the key to perceptual mislocalization. Vision Res 44(22):2605–2619PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kirsch W, Königstein E, Kunde W (2014) Action feedback affects the perception of action-related objects beyond actual action success. Front Psychol 5:17PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kirsch W, Königstein E, Kunde W (2014) Action feedback affects the perception of action-related objects beyond actual action success. Front Psychol 5:17PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kirsch W, Kunde W, Herbort O (2019) Intentional binding is unrelated to action intention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 45(3):378PubMedCrossRef Kirsch W, Kunde W, Herbort O (2019) Intentional binding is unrelated to action intention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 45(3):378PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kreegipuu K, Allik J (2004) Confusion of space and time in the flash-lag effect. Perception 33(3):293–306PubMedCrossRef Kreegipuu K, Allik J (2004) Confusion of space and time in the flash-lag effect. Perception 33(3):293–306PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Krekelberg B, Lappe M (2001) Neuronal latencies and the position of moving objects. Trends Neurosci 24(6):335–339PubMedCrossRef Krekelberg B, Lappe M (2001) Neuronal latencies and the position of moving objects. Trends Neurosci 24(6):335–339PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kulvicki J (2004) Isomorphism in information-carrying systems. Pac Philos Q 85(4):380–395CrossRef Kulvicki J (2004) Isomorphism in information-carrying systems. Pac Philos Q 85(4):380–395CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kulvicki J (2015) Analog representation and the parts principle. Rev Philos Psychol 6:165–180CrossRef Kulvicki J (2015) Analog representation and the parts principle. Rev Philos Psychol 6:165–180CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lally N, Frendo B, Diedrichsen J (2011) Sensory cancellation of self-movement facilitates visual motion detection. J vis 11(14):5–5PubMedCrossRef Lally N, Frendo B, Diedrichsen J (2011) Sensory cancellation of self-movement facilitates visual motion detection. J vis 11(14):5–5PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lamotte M, Izaute M, Droit-Volet S (2012) Awareness of time distortions and its relation with time judgment: a metacognitive approach. Conscious Cogn 21(2):835–842PubMedCrossRef Lamotte M, Izaute M, Droit-Volet S (2012) Awareness of time distortions and its relation with time judgment: a metacognitive approach. Conscious Cogn 21(2):835–842PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lappin JS, Tadin D, Nyquist JB, Corn AL (2009) Spatial and temporal limits of motion perception across variations in speed, eccentricity, and low vision. J vis 9(1):30–30PubMedCrossRef Lappin JS, Tadin D, Nyquist JB, Corn AL (2009) Spatial and temporal limits of motion perception across variations in speed, eccentricity, and low vision. J vis 9(1):30–30PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Libet B, Gleason CA, Wright EW, Pearl DK (1983) Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential) the unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 106(3):623–642PubMedCrossRef Libet B, Gleason CA, Wright EW, Pearl DK (1983) Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential) the unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 106(3):623–642PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Linares D, Holcombe AO, White AL (2009) Where is the moving object now? Judgments of instantaneous position show poor temporal precision (SD= 70 ms). J vis 9(13):9–9PubMedCrossRef Linares D, Holcombe AO, White AL (2009) Where is the moving object now? Judgments of instantaneous position show poor temporal precision (SD= 70 ms). J vis 9(13):9–9PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lisi M, Cavanagh P (2015) Dissociation between the perceptual and saccadic localization of moving objects. Curr Biol 25(19):2535–2540PubMedCrossRef Lisi M, Cavanagh P (2015) Dissociation between the perceptual and saccadic localization of moving objects. Curr Biol 25(19):2535–2540PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lubashevsky I (2019) Psychophysical laws as reflection of mental space properties. Phys Life Rev 31:276–303PubMedCrossRef Lubashevsky I (2019) Psychophysical laws as reflection of mental space properties. Phys Life Rev 31:276–303PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Makwana M, Srinivasan N (2019) Self-associated stimuli produce stronger intentional binding. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 45(11):1436PubMedCrossRef Makwana M, Srinivasan N (2019) Self-associated stimuli produce stronger intentional binding. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 45(11):1436PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Matthews N, Welch L, Achtman R, Fenton R, FitzGerald B (2016) Simultaneity and temporal order judgments exhibit distinct reaction times and training effects. PLoS ONE 11(1):e0145926PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Matthews N, Welch L, Achtman R, Fenton R, FitzGerald B (2016) Simultaneity and temporal order judgments exhibit distinct reaction times and training effects. PLoS ONE 11(1):e0145926PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Maus GW, Khurana B, Nijhawan R (2010) History and theory of flash-lag: Past, present, and future. In: Nijhawan R, Khurana B (eds) Space and time in perception and action. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, pp 477–499CrossRef Maus GW, Khurana B, Nijhawan R (2010) History and theory of flash-lag: Past, present, and future. In: Nijhawan R, Khurana B (eds) Space and time in perception and action. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, pp 477–499CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Miller J, Schwarz W (2006) Dissociations between reaction times and temporal order judgments: a diffusion model approach. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(2):394PubMedCrossRef Miller J, Schwarz W (2006) Dissociations between reaction times and temporal order judgments: a diffusion model approach. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(2):394PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Miller J, Vieweg P, Kruize N, McLea B (2010) Subjective reports of stimulus, response, and decision times in speeded tasks: how accurate are decision time reports? Conscious Cogn 19(4):1013–1036PubMedCrossRef Miller J, Vieweg P, Kruize N, McLea B (2010) Subjective reports of stimulus, response, and decision times in speeded tasks: how accurate are decision time reports? Conscious Cogn 19(4):1013–1036PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mitani K, Kashino M (2017) Auditory feedback assists post-hoc error correction of temporal reproduction, and perception of self-produced time intervals in subsecond range. Front Psychol 8:2325PubMedCrossRef Mitani K, Kashino M (2017) Auditory feedback assists post-hoc error correction of temporal reproduction, and perception of self-produced time intervals in subsecond range. Front Psychol 8:2325PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Moore JW, Obhi SS (2012) Intentional binding and the sense of agency: a review. Conscious Cogn 21(1):546–561PubMedCrossRef Moore JW, Obhi SS (2012) Intentional binding and the sense of agency: a review. Conscious Cogn 21(1):546–561PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Moore JW, Middleton D, Haggard P, Fletcher PC (2012) Exploring implicit and explicit aspects of sense of agency. Conscious Cogn 21(4):1748–1753PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Moore JW, Middleton D, Haggard P, Fletcher PC (2012) Exploring implicit and explicit aspects of sense of agency. Conscious Cogn 21(4):1748–1753PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Moore JW, Teufel C, Subramaniam N, Davis G, Fletcher PC (2013) Attribution of intentional causation influences the perception of observed movements: behavioral evidence and neural correlates. Front Psychol 4:23PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Moore JW, Teufel C, Subramaniam N, Davis G, Fletcher PC (2013) Attribution of intentional causation influences the perception of observed movements: behavioral evidence and neural correlates. Front Psychol 4:23PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Motes MA, Hubbard TL, Courtney JR, Rypma B (2008) A principal components analysis of dynamic spatial memory biases. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 34(5):1076PubMedCrossRef Motes MA, Hubbard TL, Courtney JR, Rypma B (2008) A principal components analysis of dynamic spatial memory biases. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 34(5):1076PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Murakami I (2001) The flash-lag effect as a spatiotemporal correlation structure. J vis 1(2):6–6CrossRef Murakami I (2001) The flash-lag effect as a spatiotemporal correlation structure. J vis 1(2):6–6CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler J, Stork S, Kerzel D (2002) Comparing mislocalizations with moving stimuli: the Fröhlich effect, the flash-lag, and representational momentum. Vis Cogn 9(1–2):120–138CrossRef Müsseler J, Stork S, Kerzel D (2002) Comparing mislocalizations with moving stimuli: the Fröhlich effect, the flash-lag, and representational momentum. Vis Cogn 9(1–2):120–138CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nijhawan R (2008) Visual prediction: psychophysics and neurophysiology of compensation for time delays. Behav Brain Sci 31(2):179–198PubMedCrossRef Nijhawan R (2008) Visual prediction: psychophysics and neurophysiology of compensation for time delays. Behav Brain Sci 31(2):179–198PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nijhawan R, Khurana B (2003) The flash-lag effect during voluntary and involuntary limb movements. J vis 3(9):542CrossRef Nijhawan R, Khurana B (2003) The flash-lag effect during voluntary and involuntary limb movements. J vis 3(9):542CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nolden S, Haering C, Kiesel A (2012) Assessing intentional binding with the method of constant stimuli. Conscious Cogn 21(3):1176–1185PubMedCrossRef Nolden S, Haering C, Kiesel A (2012) Assessing intentional binding with the method of constant stimuli. Conscious Cogn 21(3):1176–1185PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Obhi SS, Hall P (2011) Sense of agency in joint action: influence of human and computer co-actors. Exp Brain Res 211(3–4):663–670PubMedCrossRef Obhi SS, Hall P (2011) Sense of agency in joint action: influence of human and computer co-actors. Exp Brain Res 211(3–4):663–670PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pacherie E (2010) Self-agency. In: Gallagher S (ed) The Oxford Handbook of the Self. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 440–462 Pacherie E (2010) Self-agency. In: Gallagher S (ed) The Oxford Handbook of the Self. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 440–462
Zurück zum Zitat Patel SS, Ogmen H, Bedell HE, Sampath V (2000) Flash-lag effect: differential latency, not postdiction. Science 290(5494):1051–1051PubMedCrossRef Patel SS, Ogmen H, Bedell HE, Sampath V (2000) Flash-lag effect: differential latency, not postdiction. Science 290(5494):1051–1051PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Peacocke C (2001) Does perception have a nonconceptual content? J Philos 98(5):239–264CrossRef Peacocke C (2001) Does perception have a nonconceptual content? J Philos 98(5):239–264CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Penney TB, Cheng X (2018) Duration bisection: a user’s guide. In: Vatakis A, Balcı F, Di Luca M, Correa Á (eds) Timing and time perception: procedures, measures, & applications. Brill, Leiden, The Netherlands, pp 98–127 Penney TB, Cheng X (2018) Duration bisection: a user’s guide. In: Vatakis A, Balcı F, Di Luca M, Correa Á (eds) Timing and time perception: procedures, measures, & applications. Brill, Leiden, The Netherlands, pp 98–127
Zurück zum Zitat Petzschner FH, Glasauer S, Stephan KE (2015) A Bayesian perspective on magnitude estimation. Trends Cogn Sci 19(5):285–293PubMedCrossRef Petzschner FH, Glasauer S, Stephan KE (2015) A Bayesian perspective on magnitude estimation. Trends Cogn Sci 19(5):285–293PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Phillips B (2017) The Shifting Border Between Perception and Cognition. Noûs 53(2):316–346CrossRef Phillips B (2017) The Shifting Border Between Perception and Cognition. Noûs 53(2):316–346CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Poonian SK, Cunnington R (2013) Intentional binding in self-made and observed actions. Exp Brain Res 229(3):419–427PubMedCrossRef Poonian SK, Cunnington R (2013) Intentional binding in self-made and observed actions. Exp Brain Res 229(3):419–427PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Quilty-Dunn J (2016) Iconicity and the format of perception. J Conscious Stud 23(3–4):255–263 Quilty-Dunn J (2016) Iconicity and the format of perception. J Conscious Stud 23(3–4):255–263
Zurück zum Zitat Raftopoulos A (2009) Cognition and perception: how do psychology and neural science inform philosophy? MIT Press, Cambridge, MACrossRef Raftopoulos A (2009) Cognition and perception: how do psychology and neural science inform philosophy? MIT Press, Cambridge, MACrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rammsayer TH, Buttkus F, Altenmüller E (2012) Musicians do better than nonmusicians in both auditory and visual timing tasks. Music Percept: Interdisciplinary J 30(1):85–96CrossRef Rammsayer TH, Buttkus F, Altenmüller E (2012) Musicians do better than nonmusicians in both auditory and visual timing tasks. Music Percept: Interdisciplinary J 30(1):85–96CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Repp BH (2011) No temporal binding of action consequences to actions in a rhythmic context. Exp Brain Res 214(4):491PubMedCrossRef Repp BH (2011) No temporal binding of action consequences to actions in a rhythmic context. Exp Brain Res 214(4):491PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Reznik D, Henkin Y, Levy O, Mukamel R (2015) Perceived loudness of self-generated sounds is differentially modified by expected sound intensity. PLoS ONE 10(5):e0127651PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Reznik D, Henkin Y, Levy O, Mukamel R (2015) Perceived loudness of self-generated sounds is differentially modified by expected sound intensity. PLoS ONE 10(5):e0127651PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat De Sá Teixeira NDS (2016) How fast do objects fall in visual memory? Uncovering the temporal and spatial features of representational gravity. PLoS ONE 11(2):e0148953PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef De Sá Teixeira NDS (2016) How fast do objects fall in visual memory? Uncovering the temporal and spatial features of representational gravity. PLoS ONE 11(2):e0148953PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Saradjian AH (2015) Sensory modulation of movement, posture and locomotion. Neurophysiol Clin/clin Neurophysiol 45(4–5):255–267CrossRef Saradjian AH (2015) Sensory modulation of movement, posture and locomotion. Neurophysiol Clin/clin Neurophysiol 45(4–5):255–267CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sarrazin JC, Cleeremans A, Haggard P (2008) How do we know what we are doing? Time, intention and awareness of action. Conscious Cogn 17(3):602–615PubMedCrossRef Sarrazin JC, Cleeremans A, Haggard P (2008) How do we know what we are doing? Time, intention and awareness of action. Conscious Cogn 17(3):602–615PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sato A (2008) Action observation modulates auditory perception of the consequence of others’ actions. Conscious Cogn 17(4):1219–1227PubMedCrossRef Sato A (2008) Action observation modulates auditory perception of the consequence of others’ actions. Conscious Cogn 17(4):1219–1227PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schwarz KA, Pfister R, Kluge M, Weller L, Kunde W (2017) Do we see it or not? Sensory attenuation in the visual domain. J Exp Psychol Gen 147(3):418PubMedCrossRef Schwarz KA, Pfister R, Kluge M, Weller L, Kunde W (2017) Do we see it or not? Sensory attenuation in the visual domain. J Exp Psychol Gen 147(3):418PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schwarz KA, Weller L, Pfister R, Kunde W (2019) Connecting action control and agency: does action-effect binding affect temporal binding? Conscious Cogn 76:102833PubMedCrossRef Schwarz KA, Weller L, Pfister R, Kunde W (2019) Connecting action control and agency: does action-effect binding affect temporal binding? Conscious Cogn 76:102833PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Seifried T, Ulrich R, Bausenhart KM, Rolke B, Osman A (2010) Temporal preparation decreases perceptual latency: evidence from a clock paradigm. Q J Exp Psychol 63(12):2432–2451CrossRef Seifried T, Ulrich R, Bausenhart KM, Rolke B, Osman A (2010) Temporal preparation decreases perceptual latency: evidence from a clock paradigm. Q J Exp Psychol 63(12):2432–2451CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Smithson, M (2012). Uncertainty. In V.S. Ramachandran (Ed.) Encyclopedia of Human Behavior (pp. 621–628), 2nd Edition. Oxford: Elsevier. Smithson, M (2012). Uncertainty. In V.S. Ramachandran (Ed.) Encyclopedia of Human Behavior (pp. 621–628), 2nd Edition. Oxford: Elsevier.
Zurück zum Zitat Stevens SS (1975) Psychophysics: Introduction to its Perceptual, Neural, and Social Prospects. Wiley, New York Stevens SS (1975) Psychophysics: Introduction to its Perceptual, Neural, and Social Prospects. Wiley, New York
Zurück zum Zitat Strother L, House KA, Obhi SS (2010) Subjective agency and awareness of shared actions. Conscious Cogn 19(1):12–20PubMedCrossRef Strother L, House KA, Obhi SS (2010) Subjective agency and awareness of shared actions. Conscious Cogn 19(1):12–20PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Suzuki K, Lush P, Seth A, Roseboom W (2019) Intentional binding without intentional action. Psychol Sci 30(6):842–853PubMedCrossRef Suzuki K, Lush P, Seth A, Roseboom W (2019) Intentional binding without intentional action. Psychol Sci 30(6):842–853PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Synofzik M, Vosgerau G, Newen A (2008a) I move, therefore I am: a new theoretical framework to investigate agency and ownership. Conscious Cogn 17(2):411–424PubMedCrossRef Synofzik M, Vosgerau G, Newen A (2008a) I move, therefore I am: a new theoretical framework to investigate agency and ownership. Conscious Cogn 17(2):411–424PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Synofzik M, Vosgerau G, Newen A (2008b) Beyond the comparator model: a multifactorial two-step account of agency. Conscious Cogn 17(1):219–239PubMedCrossRef Synofzik M, Vosgerau G, Newen A (2008b) Beyond the comparator model: a multifactorial two-step account of agency. Conscious Cogn 17(1):219–239PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tanaka H, Homma K, Imamizu H (2012) Illusory reversal of causality between touch and vision has no effect on prism adaptation rate. Front Psychol 3:545PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Tanaka H, Homma K, Imamizu H (2012) Illusory reversal of causality between touch and vision has no effect on prism adaptation rate. Front Psychol 3:545PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tsakiris M, Haggard P (2005) Experimenting with the acting self. Cogn Neuropsychol 22(3–4):387–407PubMedCrossRef Tsakiris M, Haggard P (2005) Experimenting with the acting self. Cogn Neuropsychol 22(3–4):387–407PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tversky A, Kahneman D (1974) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 185(4157):1124–1131CrossRef Tversky A, Kahneman D (1974) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 185(4157):1124–1131CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tye M (2006) The thesis of nonconceptual content. Eur Rev Philos 6(6):7–30 Tye M (2006) The thesis of nonconceptual content. Eur Rev Philos 6(6):7–30
Zurück zum Zitat Vastano R, Deschrijver E, Pozzo T, Brass M (2018) Temporal binding effect in the action observation domain: evidence from an action-based somatosensory paradigm. Conscious Cogn 60:1–8PubMedCrossRef Vastano R, Deschrijver E, Pozzo T, Brass M (2018) Temporal binding effect in the action observation domain: evidence from an action-based somatosensory paradigm. Conscious Cogn 60:1–8PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Vinson DW, Abney DH, Dale R, Matlock T (2014) High-level context effects on spatial displacement: the effects of body orientation and language on memory. Front Psychol 5:637PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Vinson DW, Abney DH, Dale R, Matlock T (2014) High-level context effects on spatial displacement: the effects of body orientation and language on memory. Front Psychol 5:637PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Vinson DW, Engelen J, Zwaan RA, Matlock T, Dale R (2017) Implied motion language can influence visual spatial memory. Mem Cognit 45(5):852–862PubMedCrossRef Vinson DW, Engelen J, Zwaan RA, Matlock T, Dale R (2017) Implied motion language can influence visual spatial memory. Mem Cognit 45(5):852–862PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat von Holst E, Mittelstaedt H (1950) Das Reafferenzprinzip. Wechselwirkungen zwischen zentralnervensystem und Peripherie. Naturwissenschaften 37:464–476CrossRef von Holst E, Mittelstaedt H (1950) Das Reafferenzprinzip. Wechselwirkungen zwischen zentralnervensystem und Peripherie. Naturwissenschaften 37:464–476CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Vreven D, Verghese P (2005) Predictability and the dynamics of position processing in the flash-lag effect. Perception 34(1):31–44PubMedCrossRef Vreven D, Verghese P (2005) Predictability and the dynamics of position processing in the flash-lag effect. Perception 34(1):31–44PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Vuorre M (2017) On time, causation, and the sense of agency. J Conscious Stud 24(3–4):203–215 Vuorre M (2017) On time, causation, and the sense of agency. J Conscious Stud 24(3–4):203–215
Zurück zum Zitat Walsh V (2003) A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn Sci 7(11):483–488PubMedCrossRef Walsh V (2003) A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn Sci 7(11):483–488PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wang Y, Damen TG, Aarts H (2017) Uncovering effects of self-control and stimulus-driven action selection on the sense of agency. Conscious Cogn 55:245–253PubMedCrossRef Wang Y, Damen TG, Aarts H (2017) Uncovering effects of self-control and stimulus-driven action selection on the sense of agency. Conscious Cogn 55:245–253PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wearden J (2014) Modelling behaviour on classical timing tasks: Verbal estimation, production, and reproduction. Procedia Soc Behav Sci 126:34–35CrossRef Wearden J (2014) Modelling behaviour on classical timing tasks: Verbal estimation, production, and reproduction. Procedia Soc Behav Sci 126:34–35CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wegner DM (2004) Précis of the illusion of conscious will. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(5):649–659CrossRef Wegner DM (2004) Précis of the illusion of conscious will. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(5):649–659CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Weiss C, Herwig A, Schütz-Bosbach S (2011a) The self in action effects: selective attenuation of self-generated sounds. Cognition 121(2):207–218PubMedCrossRef Weiss C, Herwig A, Schütz-Bosbach S (2011a) The self in action effects: selective attenuation of self-generated sounds. Cognition 121(2):207–218PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Weiss C, Herwig A, Schütz-Bosbach S (2011b) The self in social interactions: sensory attenuation of auditory action effects is stronger in interactions with others. PLoS ONE 6(7):e22723PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Weiss C, Herwig A, Schütz-Bosbach S (2011b) The self in social interactions: sensory attenuation of auditory action effects is stronger in interactions with others. PLoS ONE 6(7):e22723PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Weller L, Schwarz KA, Kunde W, Pfister R (2020) Something from nothing: Agency for deliberate nonactions. Cognition 196:104136PubMedCrossRef Weller L, Schwarz KA, Kunde W, Pfister R (2020) Something from nothing: Agency for deliberate nonactions. Cognition 196:104136PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wen W, Brann E, Di Costa S, Haggard P (2018) Enhanced perceptual processing of self-generated motion: evidence from steady-state visual evoked potentials. Neuroimage 175:438–448PubMedCrossRef Wen W, Brann E, Di Costa S, Haggard P (2018) Enhanced perceptual processing of self-generated motion: evidence from steady-state visual evoked potentials. Neuroimage 175:438–448PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wenke D, Waszak F, Haggard P (2009) Action selection and action awareness. Psychol Res PRPF 73(4):602–612CrossRef Wenke D, Waszak F, Haggard P (2009) Action selection and action awareness. Psychol Res PRPF 73(4):602–612CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat White AL, Linares D, Holcombe AO (2008) Visuomotor timing compensates for changes in perceptual latency. Curr Biol 18(20):R951–R953PubMedCrossRef White AL, Linares D, Holcombe AO (2008) Visuomotor timing compensates for changes in perceptual latency. Curr Biol 18(20):R951–R953PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wohlschläger A, Engbert K, Haggard P (2003a) Intentionality as a constituting condition for the own self–and other selves. Conscious Cogn 12(4):708–716PubMedCrossRef Wohlschläger A, Engbert K, Haggard P (2003a) Intentionality as a constituting condition for the own self–and other selves. Conscious Cogn 12(4):708–716PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wohlschläger A, Haggard P, Gesierich B, Prinz W (2003b) The perceived onset time of self-and other-generated actions. Psychol Sci 14(6):586–591PubMedCrossRef Wohlschläger A, Haggard P, Gesierich B, Prinz W (2003b) The perceived onset time of self-and other-generated actions. Psychol Sci 14(6):586–591PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wu W (2013) Visual spatial constancy and modularity: Does intention penetrate vision? Philos Stud 165(2):647–669CrossRef Wu W (2013) Visual spatial constancy and modularity: Does intention penetrate vision? Philos Stud 165(2):647–669CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Yarrow K, Obhi SS (2014) Temporal perception in the context of action. In: Arstila V, Lloyd D (eds) Subjective time: the philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of temporality. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 455–476 Yarrow K, Obhi SS (2014) Temporal perception in the context of action. In: Arstila V, Lloyd D (eds) Subjective time: the philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of temporality. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 455–476
Zurück zum Zitat Yokosaka T, Kuroki S, Nishida SY, Watanabe J (2015) Apparent time interval of visual stimuli is compressed during fast hand movement. PLoS ONE 10(4):e0124901PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Yokosaka T, Kuroki S, Nishida SY, Watanabe J (2015) Apparent time interval of visual stimuli is compressed during fast hand movement. PLoS ONE 10(4):e0124901PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Yon D, Press C (2017) Predicted action consequences are perceptually facilitated before cancellation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 43(6):1073PubMedCrossRef Yon D, Press C (2017) Predicted action consequences are perceptually facilitated before cancellation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 43(6):1073PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Yon D, Press C (2018) Sensory predictions during action support perception of imitative reactions across suprasecond delays. Cognition 173:21–27PubMedCrossRef Yon D, Press C (2018) Sensory predictions during action support perception of imitative reactions across suprasecond delays. Cognition 173:21–27PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
The implicit sense of agency is not a perceptual effect but is a judgment effect
verfasst von
Nagireddy Neelakanteswar Reddy
Publikationsdatum
09.11.2021
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Cognitive Processing / Ausgabe 1/2022
Print ISSN: 1612-4782
Elektronische ISSN: 1612-4790
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-021-01066-x

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2022

Cognitive Processing 1/2022 Zur Ausgabe

Neuer Inhalt