Skip to main content

2017 | OriginalPaper | Buchkapitel

2. Visuelle Informationsverarbeitung

verfasst von : Prof. Dr. Jochen Müsseler

Erschienen in: Allgemeine Psychologie

Verlag: Springer Berlin Heidelberg

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Zusammenfassung

Visuelle Wahrnehmungsprozesse kennzeichnen den bedeutendsten Sinn des Menschen, den Sehsinn bzw. Gesichtssinn. In der wissenschaftlichen Auseinandersetzung mit diesem Thema wird untersucht, wie die Informationen unserer physikalischen Umwelt (distaler Reiz) von den Sinnesrezeptoren des Auges (proximaler Reiz) aufgenommen und kognitiv verarbeitet werden, um so den Wahrnehmungseindruck hervorzurufen, der unser Erleben und Verhalten maßgeblich beeinflusst. Das vorliegende Kapitel beschreibt die Prozesse der elementaren Encodierungsprozesse bis hin zu den Prozessen der Objektidentifizierung und schließt mit einer kurzen Skizzierung der wichtigsten Wahrnehmungstheorien.
Schlüsselwörter: Visuelle Wahrnehmung; Farbwahrnehmung; Raum- und Tiefenwahrnehmung; Bewegungswahrnehmung; Objektwahrnehmung; Psychophysik; Gestaltwahrnehmung; Ökologische Wahrnehmung

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 390 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe




 

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Algom, D., & Cohen-Raz, L. (1987). Sensory and cognitive factors in the processing of visual velocity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 3–13. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.13.1.3.PubMed Algom, D., & Cohen-Raz, L. (1987). Sensory and cognitive factors in the processing of visual velocity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 3–13. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.13.1.3.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Badcock, D. R., & Westheimer, G. (1985). Spatial location and hyperacuity: The centre/surround localization contribution function has two substrates. Vision Research, 25, 1259–1267. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(85)90041-0.PubMedCrossRef Badcock, D. R., & Westheimer, G. (1985). Spatial location and hyperacuity: The centre/surround localization contribution function has two substrates. Vision Research, 25, 1259–1267. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(85)90041-0.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Baldo, M. V. C., Ranvaud, R. D., & Morya, E. (2002). Flag errors in soccer games: the flash-lag effect brought to real life. Perception, 31, 1205–1210. doi:10.1068/p3422.PubMedCrossRef Baldo, M. V. C., Ranvaud, R. D., & Morya, E. (2002). Flag errors in soccer games: the flash-lag effect brought to real life. Perception, 31, 1205–1210. doi:10.1068/p3422.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Barlow, H. B., & Hill, R. M. (1963). Evidence for a physiological explanation of the waterfall phenomenon and figural after-effects. Nature, 200(200), 1345–1347. doi:10.1038/2001345a0.PubMedCrossRef Barlow, H. B., & Hill, R. M. (1963). Evidence for a physiological explanation of the waterfall phenomenon and figural after-effects. Nature, 200(200), 1345–1347. doi:10.1038/2001345a0.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bear, M. F., Conners, B. W., & Paradiso, M. A. (2009). Neurowissenschaften. Ein grundlegendes Lehrbuch für Biologie, Medizin und Psychologie. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischr Verlag, Springer. Bear, M. F., Conners, B. W., & Paradiso, M. A. (2009). Neurowissenschaften. Ein grundlegendes Lehrbuch für Biologie, Medizin und Psychologie. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischr Verlag, Springer.
Zurück zum Zitat Beck, J. (1966). Effect of orientation and shape similarity in perceptual grouping. Perception and Psychophysics, 1, 300–302. doi:10.3758/BF03215792.CrossRef Beck, J. (1966). Effect of orientation and shape similarity in perceptual grouping. Perception and Psychophysics, 1, 300–302. doi:10.3758/BF03215792.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Berry, M. J., Brivanlou, I. H., Jordan, T. A., & Meister, M. (1999). Anticipation of moving stimuli by the retina. Nature, 398(6725), 334–338. doi:10.1038/18678.PubMedCrossRef Berry, M. J., Brivanlou, I. H., Jordan, T. A., & Meister, M. (1999). Anticipation of moving stimuli by the retina. Nature, 398(6725), 334–338. doi:10.1038/18678.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115–147. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.2.115.PubMedCrossRef Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115–147. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.2.115.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Biederman, I. (1995). Visual object recognition. In S. M. Kosslyn, & D. N. Osherson (Hrsg.), Visual cognition: An invitation to cognitive science (S. 121–165). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Biederman, I. (1995). Visual object recognition. In S. M. Kosslyn, & D. N. Osherson (Hrsg.), Visual cognition: An invitation to cognitive science (S. 121–165). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Biederman, I. (2000). Recognizing depth-rotated objects: A review of recent research and theory. Spatial Vision, 13, 241–253. doi:10.1163/156856800741063.PubMedCrossRef Biederman, I. (2000). Recognizing depth-rotated objects: A review of recent research and theory. Spatial Vision, 13, 241–253. doi:10.1163/156856800741063.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bocianski, D., Müsseler, J., & Erlhagen, W. (2008). Relative mislocalization of successively presented stimuli. Vision Research, 48, 2204–2212. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2008.06.016.PubMedCrossRef Bocianski, D., Müsseler, J., & Erlhagen, W. (2008). Relative mislocalization of successively presented stimuli. Vision Research, 48, 2204–2212. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2008.06.016.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bocianski, D., Müsseler, J., & Erlhagen, W. (2010). Effects of attention on a relative mislocalization with successively presented stimuli. Vision Research, 50, 1793–1802. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.05.036.PubMedCrossRef Bocianski, D., Müsseler, J., & Erlhagen, W. (2010). Effects of attention on a relative mislocalization with successively presented stimuli. Vision Research, 50, 1793–1802. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.05.036.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Borst, A., Haag, J., & Reiff, D. F. (2010). Fly motion vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33, 49–70. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153155.PubMedCrossRef Borst, A., Haag, J., & Reiff, D. F. (2010). Fly motion vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33, 49–70. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153155.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brown, J. F. (1931). The visual perception of velocity. Psychologische Forschung, 14, 199–232. doi:10.1007/BF00403873.CrossRef Brown, J. F. (1931). The visual perception of velocity. Psychologische Forschung, 14, 199–232. doi:10.1007/BF00403873.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bruce, V., Green, P. R., & Georgeson, M. A. (1996). Visual perception. Physiology, psychology, and ecology. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press. Bruce, V., Green, P. R., & Georgeson, M. A. (1996). Visual perception. Physiology, psychology, and ecology. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64, 123–152. doi:10.1037/h0043805.PubMedCrossRef Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64, 123–152. doi:10.1037/h0043805.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Buchner, A., Brandt, M., Bell, R., & Weise, J. (2006). Car backlight position and fog density bias observer-car distance estimates and time-to-collision judgments. Human Factors, 48, 300–317.PubMedCrossRef Buchner, A., Brandt, M., Bell, R., & Weise, J. (2006). Car backlight position and fog density bias observer-car distance estimates and time-to-collision judgments. Human Factors, 48, 300–317.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cowey, A., & Rolls, E. T. (1974). Human cortical magnification factor and its relation to visual acuity. Experimental Brain Research, 21, 447–454. doi:10.1007/BF00237163.PubMedCrossRef Cowey, A., & Rolls, E. T. (1974). Human cortical magnification factor and its relation to visual acuity. Experimental Brain Research, 21, 447–454. doi:10.1007/BF00237163.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dartnall, H. J. A., Bowmaker, J. K., & Mollon, J. D. (1983). Human visual pigments: Microspetrophometric results from the eyes of seven persons. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 220B, 115–130. doi:10.1098/rspb.1983.0091.CrossRef Dartnall, H. J. A., Bowmaker, J. K., & Mollon, J. D. (1983). Human visual pigments: Microspetrophometric results from the eyes of seven persons. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 220B, 115–130. doi:10.1098/rspb.1983.0091.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat DeAngelis, G. C. (2000). Seeing in three dimensions: the neurophysiology of stereopsis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 80–90. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01443-6.PubMedCrossRef DeAngelis, G. C. (2000). Seeing in three dimensions: the neurophysiology of stereopsis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 80–90. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01443-6.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat DeValois, R. L., & Jacobs, G. H. (1968). Primate color vision. Science, 162, 533–540. doi:10.1126/science.162.3853.533.CrossRef DeValois, R. L., & Jacobs, G. H. (1968). Primate color vision. Science, 162, 533–540. doi:10.1126/science.162.3853.533.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat DeYoe, E. A., & Van Essen, D. C. (1988). Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex. Trends in Neuorscience, 11, 219–226. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(88)90130-0.CrossRef DeYoe, E. A., & Van Essen, D. C. (1988). Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex. Trends in Neuorscience, 11, 219–226. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(88)90130-0.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ditzinger, T. (2006). Illusionen des Sehens. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag. Ditzinger, T. (2006). Illusionen des Sehens. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.
Zurück zum Zitat Dowling, J. E., & Boycott, B. B. (1966). Organization of the primate retina: electron microscopy. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 166B, 80–111. doi:10.1098/rspb.1966.0086.CrossRef Dowling, J. E., & Boycott, B. B. (1966). Organization of the primate retina: electron microscopy. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 166B, 80–111. doi:10.1098/rspb.1966.0086.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Drasdo, N. (1977). The neural representation of visual space. Nature, 266, 552–556. doi:10.1038/266554a0.CrossRef Drasdo, N. (1977). The neural representation of visual space. Nature, 266, 552–556. doi:10.1038/266554a0.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Driver, J., & Baylis, G. C. (1995). One-sided edge assignment in vision. II. Part decomposition, shape description, and attention to objects. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 201–206. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772645.CrossRef Driver, J., & Baylis, G. C. (1995). One-sided edge assignment in vision. II. Part decomposition, shape description, and attention to objects. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 201–206. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772645.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Driver, J., & Baylis, G. C. (1996). Edge-assignment and figure-ground segmentation in short-term visual matching. Cognitive Psychology, 31, 248–306. doi:10.1006/cogp.1996.0018.PubMedCrossRef Driver, J., & Baylis, G. C. (1996). Edge-assignment and figure-ground segmentation in short-term visual matching. Cognitive Psychology, 31, 248–306. doi:10.1006/cogp.1996.0018.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Epstein, W., & Rogers, S. J. (Hrsg.). (1995). Perception of space and motion. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.. Epstein, W., & Rogers, S. J. (Hrsg.). (1995). Perception of space and motion. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc..
Zurück zum Zitat Erlhagen, W. (2003). Internal models for visual perception. Biological Cybernetics, 88, 409–417. doi:10.1007/s00422-002-0387-1.PubMedCrossRef Erlhagen, W. (2003). Internal models for visual perception. Biological Cybernetics, 88, 409–417. doi:10.1007/s00422-002-0387-1.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Erlhagen, W., & Jancke, D. (2004). The role of action plans and other cognitive factors in motion extrapolation: A modelling study. Visual Cognition, 11, 315–340. doi:10.1080/13506280344000293.CrossRef Erlhagen, W., & Jancke, D. (2004). The role of action plans and other cognitive factors in motion extrapolation: A modelling study. Visual Cognition, 11, 315–340. doi:10.1080/13506280344000293.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Erlhagen, W., Bastian, A., Jancke, D., Riehle, A., & Schöner, G. (1999). The distribution of neuronal population activation (DPA) as a tool to study interaction and integration in cortical representations. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 94, 53–66. doi:10.1016/S0165-0270(99)00125-9.PubMedCrossRef Erlhagen, W., Bastian, A., Jancke, D., Riehle, A., & Schöner, G. (1999). The distribution of neuronal population activation (DPA) as a tool to study interaction and integration in cortical representations. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 94, 53–66. doi:10.1016/S0165-0270(99)00125-9.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fechner, G. T. (1860). Elemente der Psychophysik. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel. Fechner, G. T. (1860). Elemente der Psychophysik. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel.
Zurück zum Zitat Felleman, D. J., & Van Essen, D. C. (1991). Distributed hierachical processing in the primate visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1, 1–47. doi:10.1093/cercor/1.1.1-a.PubMedCrossRef Felleman, D. J., & Van Essen, D. C. (1991). Distributed hierachical processing in the primate visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1, 1–47. doi:10.1093/cercor/1.1.1-a.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Finney, D. J. (1971). Probit analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Finney, D. J. (1971). Probit analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Flannagan, M. J., Sivak, M., Schumann, J., Traube, E. C., & Kojima, S. (1997). Distance perception in driverside and passenger-side convex rearview mirrors: Objects in mirrors are more complicated than they appear. Bd. UMTRI-97–32. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Transportation, Research Institute. Flannagan, M. J., Sivak, M., Schumann, J., Traube, E. C., & Kojima, S. (1997). Distance perception in driverside and passenger-side convex rearview mirrors: Objects in mirrors are more complicated than they appear. Bd. UMTRI-97–32. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Transportation, Research Institute.
Zurück zum Zitat Freyd, J. J., & Finke, R. A. (1984). Representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 126–132. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.10.1.126. Freyd, J. J., & Finke, R. A. (1984). Representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 126–132. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.10.1.126.
Zurück zum Zitat Fröhlich, F. W. (1923). Über die Messung der Empfindungszeit. Zeitschrift für Sinnesphysiologie, 54, 58–78. doi:10.1007/bf01723521. Fröhlich, F. W. (1923). Über die Messung der Empfindungszeit. Zeitschrift für Sinnesphysiologie, 54, 58–78. doi:10.1007/bf01723521.
Zurück zum Zitat Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2012). Farbwahrnehmung und ihre Störungen. In H. O. Karnath, & P. Thier (Hrsg.), Kognitive Neurowissenschaften (S. 45–52). Berlin: Springer Verlag.CrossRef Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2012). Farbwahrnehmung und ihre Störungen. In H. O. Karnath, & P. Thier (Hrsg.), Kognitive Neurowissenschaften (S. 45–52). Berlin: Springer Verlag.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Sharpe, L. T. (2000). Color vision: from genes to perception. New York: Cambridge University Press. Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Sharpe, L. T. (2000). Color vision: from genes to perception. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J. J. (1950). The perception of the visual world. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gibson, J. J. (1950). The perception of the visual world. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Zurück zum Zitat Goebel, R., Roebroeck, A., Kim, D.-S., & Formisano, E. (2003). Investigating directed cortical interactions in time-resolved fMRI data using vector autoregressive modeling and Granger causality mapping. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 21, 1251–1261. doi:10.1016/j.mri.2003.08.026.PubMedCrossRef Goebel, R., Roebroeck, A., Kim, D.-S., & Formisano, E. (2003). Investigating directed cortical interactions in time-resolved fMRI data using vector autoregressive modeling and Granger causality mapping. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 21, 1251–1261. doi:10.1016/j.mri.2003.08.026.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goldstein, E. B. (2015). Wahrnehmungspsychologie. Eine Einführung. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. Deutsche Ausgabe herausgegeben von K. R. Gegenfurtner Goldstein, E. B. (2015). Wahrnehmungspsychologie. Eine Einführung. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. Deutsche Ausgabe herausgegeben von K. R. Gegenfurtner
Zurück zum Zitat Gordon, I. E. (2004). Theories of visual perception. New York: Psychology Press. Gordon, I. E. (2004). Theories of visual perception. New York: Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Green, M., & Odom, J. V. (1986). Correspondence matching in apparent motion – Evidence for 3-dimensional spatial representation. Science, 233(4771), 1427–1429. doi:10.1126/science.3749887.PubMedCrossRef Green, M., & Odom, J. V. (1986). Correspondence matching in apparent motion – Evidence for 3-dimensional spatial representation. Science, 233(4771), 1427–1429. doi:10.1126/science.3749887.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grunewald, A., & Lankheet, M. J. M. (1996). Orthogonal motion after-effect illusion predicted by a model of cortical motion processing. Nature, 384(6607), 358–360. doi:10.1038/384358a0.PubMedCrossRef Grunewald, A., & Lankheet, M. J. M. (1996). Orthogonal motion after-effect illusion predicted by a model of cortical motion processing. Nature, 384(6607), 358–360. doi:10.1038/384358a0.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grunewald, A., & Mingolla, E. (1998). Motion after-effect due to binocular sum of adaptation to linear motion. Vision Research, 38, 2963–2971. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00102-3.PubMedCrossRef Grunewald, A., & Mingolla, E. (1998). Motion after-effect due to binocular sum of adaptation to linear motion. Vision Research, 38, 2963–2971. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00102-3.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hahnel, U. J. J., & Hecht, H. (2012). The impact of rear-view mirror distance and curvature on judgements relevant to road safety. Ergonomics, 55, 23–36. doi:10.1080/00140139.2011.638402.PubMedCrossRef Hahnel, U. J. J., & Hecht, H. (2012). The impact of rear-view mirror distance and curvature on judgements relevant to road safety. Ergonomics, 55, 23–36. doi:10.1080/00140139.2011.638402.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hartline, H. K., Wagner, H. G., & Ratliff, F. (1956). Inhibition in the eye of Limulus. Journal of General Physiology, 39, 651–673. doi:10.1085/jgp.39.5.651.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hartline, H. K., Wagner, H. G., & Ratliff, F. (1956). Inhibition in the eye of Limulus. Journal of General Physiology, 39, 651–673. doi:10.1085/jgp.39.5.651.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hazelhoff, F. F., & Wiersma, H. (1924). Die Wahrnehmungszeit. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 96, 171–188. Hazelhoff, F. F., & Wiersma, H. (1924). Die Wahrnehmungszeit. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 96, 171–188.
Zurück zum Zitat Hecht, H., & Brauer, J. (2007). Convex rear view mirrors compromise distance and time-to-contact judgements. Ergonomics, 50, 601–614.PubMedCrossRef Hecht, H., & Brauer, J. (2007). Convex rear view mirrors compromise distance and time-to-contact judgements. Ergonomics, 50, 601–614.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Helmholtz, H. v. (1866/1910). Handbuch der physiologischen Optik. 3. Aufl. ergänzt und hrsg. von W. Nagel, A. Gullstrand und J. von Kries. Hamburg: Voss. Helmholtz, H. v. (1866/1910). Handbuch der physiologischen Optik. 3. Aufl. ergänzt und hrsg. von W. Nagel, A. Gullstrand und J. von Kries. Hamburg: Voss.
Zurück zum Zitat Hess, C., & Pretori, H. (1894). Messende Untersuchungen über die Gesetzmässigkeit des simultanen Helligkeits-Contrastes. Archiv für Ophthalmologie, 40, 1–24. doi:10.1007%2FBF01693963 Hess, C., & Pretori, H. (1894). Messende Untersuchungen über die Gesetzmässigkeit des simultanen Helligkeits-Contrastes. Archiv für Ophthalmologie, 40, 1–24. doi:10.1007%2FBF01693963
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard, T. L. (2005). Representational momentum and related displace-ments in spatial memory: A review of the findings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 822–851. doi:10.3758/BF03196775.CrossRef Hubbard, T. L. (2005). Representational momentum and related displace-ments in spatial memory: A review of the findings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 822–851. doi:10.3758/BF03196775.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard, T. L. (2014a). Forms of momentum across space: Representational, operational, and attentional. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 1371–1403. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0624-3.CrossRef Hubbard, T. L. (2014a). Forms of momentum across space: Representational, operational, and attentional. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 1371–1403. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0624-3.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard, T. L. (2014b). The flash-lag effect and related mislocalizations: Findings, properties, and theories. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 308–338. doi:10.1037/a0032899.PubMedCrossRef Hubbard, T. L. (2014b). The flash-lag effect and related mislocalizations: Findings, properties, and theories. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 308–338. doi:10.1037/a0032899.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1959). Receptive fields of single neurons in the cat’s striate cortex. Journal of Physiology, 148, 574–591. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1959.sp006308.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1959). Receptive fields of single neurons in the cat’s striate cortex. Journal of Physiology, 148, 574–591. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1959.sp006308.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1968). Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey straite cortex. Journal of Physiology, 195, 215–243. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008455.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1968). Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey straite cortex. Journal of Physiology, 195, 215–243. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008455.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hurvich, L. M., & Jameson, D. (2001). An opponent-process theory of color vision. In S. Yantis (Hrsg.), Visual perception (S. 129–146). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. Hurvich, L. M., & Jameson, D. (2001). An opponent-process theory of color vision. In S. Yantis (Hrsg.), Visual perception (S. 129–146). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Jancke, D., & Erlhagen, W. (2010). Bridging the gap: A model of common neural mechanisms underlying the Fröhlich effect, the flash-lag effect, and the representational momentum effect. In R. Nijhawan, & B. Khurana (Hrsg.), Space and time in perception and action. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Jancke, D., & Erlhagen, W. (2010). Bridging the gap: A model of common neural mechanisms underlying the Fröhlich effect, the flash-lag effect, and the representational momentum effect. In R. Nijhawan, & B. Khurana (Hrsg.), Space and time in perception and action. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Jäncke, L. (2005). Bildgebende Verfahren in der Psychologie und den kognitiven Neurowissenschaften. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer Verlag. Jäncke, L. (2005). Bildgebende Verfahren in der Psychologie und den kognitiven Neurowissenschaften. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer Verlag.
Zurück zum Zitat Jordan, J. S., Stork, S., Knuf, L., Kerzel, D., & Müsseler, J. (2002). Action planning affects spatial localization. In W. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Hrsg.), Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (S. 158–176). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Jordan, J. S., Stork, S., Knuf, L., Kerzel, D., & Müsseler, J. (2002). Action planning affects spatial localization. In W. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Hrsg.), Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (S. 158–176). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Julesz, B. (1971). Foundation of cyclopean perception. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Julesz, B. (1971). Foundation of cyclopean perception. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Julesz, B. (1981). Textons, the elements of texture perception and their interaction. Nature, 290, 91–97. doi:10.1038/290091a0.PubMedCrossRef Julesz, B. (1981). Textons, the elements of texture perception and their interaction. Nature, 290, 91–97. doi:10.1038/290091a0.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kahneman, D., Norman, J., & Kubovy, M. (1967). Critical duration for resolution of form – centrally or peripherally determined. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73, 323–327. doi:10.1037/h0024257.PubMedCrossRef Kahneman, D., Norman, J., & Kubovy, M. (1967). Critical duration for resolution of form – centrally or peripherally determined. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73, 323–327. doi:10.1037/h0024257.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kandel, E. R. (1996). Die Konstruktion des visuellen Bildes. In E. R. Kandel, & J. H. S. T. M. Jessel (Hrsg.), Neurowissenschaften: Eine Einführung (S. 393–411). Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag. Kandel, E. R. (1996). Die Konstruktion des visuellen Bildes. In E. R. Kandel, & J. H. S. T. M. Jessel (Hrsg.), Neurowissenschaften: Eine Einführung (S. 393–411). Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.
Zurück zum Zitat Kanisza, G. (1979). Organization in vision: Essays on Gestalt perception. New York: Praeger. Kanisza, G. (1979). Organization in vision: Essays on Gestalt perception. New York: Praeger.
Zurück zum Zitat Kehrer, L. (1997). The central performance drop in texture segmentation: a simulation based on a spatial filter model. Biological Cybernetics, 77, 297–305. doi:10.1007/s004220050391.CrossRef Kehrer, L. (1997). The central performance drop in texture segmentation: a simulation based on a spatial filter model. Biological Cybernetics, 77, 297–305. doi:10.1007/s004220050391.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kerzel, D. (2000). Eye movements and visible persistence explain the mislocalization of the final position of a moving target. Vision Research, 40, 3703–3715. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00226-1.PubMedCrossRef Kerzel, D. (2000). Eye movements and visible persistence explain the mislocalization of the final position of a moving target. Vision Research, 40, 3703–3715. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00226-1.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kerzel, D. (2010). The Fröhlich effect: Historical notes and relation to the flash-lag, current theories and reconciliation with the onset repulsion effect. In R. Nijhawan, & B. Khurana (Hrsg.), Space and Time in Perception and Action (S. 321–337). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Kerzel, D. (2010). The Fröhlich effect: Historical notes and relation to the flash-lag, current theories and reconciliation with the onset repulsion effect. In R. Nijhawan, & B. Khurana (Hrsg.), Space and Time in Perception and Action (S. 321–337). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kerzel, D., & Müsseler, J. (2002). Effects of stimulus material on the Fröhlich illusion. Vision Research, 42, 181–189. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00271-1.PubMedCrossRef Kerzel, D., & Müsseler, J. (2002). Effects of stimulus material on the Fröhlich illusion. Vision Research, 42, 181–189. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00271-1.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kerzel, D., Jordan, J. S., & Müsseler, J. (2001). The role of perceptual anticipation in the mislocalization of the final position of a moving target. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 829–840. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.27.4.829.PubMed Kerzel, D., Jordan, J. S., & Müsseler, J. (2001). The role of perceptual anticipation in the mislocalization of the final position of a moving target. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 829–840. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.27.4.829.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kimchi, R., & Navon, D. (2000). Relative judgment seems to be the key: Revisiting the Beck effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 789–805. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.26.2.789.PubMed Kimchi, R., & Navon, D. (2000). Relative judgment seems to be the key: Revisiting the Beck effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 789–805. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.26.2.789.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kirschfeld, K., & Kammer, T. (1999). The Fröhlich effect: A consequence of the interaction of visual focal attention and metacontrast. Vision Research, 39, 3702–3709. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00089-9.PubMedCrossRef Kirschfeld, K., & Kammer, T. (1999). The Fröhlich effect: A consequence of the interaction of visual focal attention and metacontrast. Vision Research, 39, 3702–3709. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00089-9.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Koenderink, J. J. (1990). The brain a geometry engine. Special Issue: Domains of mental functioning: Attempts at a synthesis. Psychological Research, 52, 122–127. doi:10.1007/BF00877519.PubMedCrossRef Koenderink, J. J. (1990). The brain a geometry engine. Special Issue: Domains of mental functioning: Attempts at a synthesis. Psychological Research, 52, 122–127. doi:10.1007/BF00877519.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Hartcourt Brace. Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Hartcourt Brace.
Zurück zum Zitat Kohler, I. (1962). Experiments with goggles. Scientific American, 206, 63–72. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0562-62.CrossRef Kohler, I. (1962). Experiments with goggles. Scientific American, 206, 63–72. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0562-62.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Köhler, W. (1958). Dynamische Zusammenhänge in der Psychologie. Bern: Huber. Köhler, W. (1958). Dynamische Zusammenhänge in der Psychologie. Bern: Huber.
Zurück zum Zitat Kolers, P. A. (1974). The illusion of movement. In R. Held, & W. Richards (Hrsg.), Perception: Mechanism and models (S. 316–323). San Francisco, CA: Freeman. Kolers, P. A. (1974). The illusion of movement. In R. Held, & W. Richards (Hrsg.), Perception: Mechanism and models (S. 316–323). San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
Zurück zum Zitat Krekelberg, B., & Lappe, M. (2000). A model of the perceived relative positions of moving objects based upon a slow averaging process. Vision Research, 40, 201–215. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00168-6.PubMedCrossRef Krekelberg, B., & Lappe, M. (2000). A model of the perceived relative positions of moving objects based upon a slow averaging process. Vision Research, 40, 201–215. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00168-6.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Krueger, L. E. (1975). Familiarity effects in visual information processing. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 949–974. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.82.6.949.PubMedCrossRef Krueger, L. E. (1975). Familiarity effects in visual information processing. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 949–974. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.82.6.949.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat LeVay, S., & Voigt, T. (1988). Ocular dominance and disparity coding in cat visual cortex. Visual Neuroscience, 1, 395–414. doi:10.1017/S0952523800004168.PubMedCrossRef LeVay, S., & Voigt, T. (1988). Ocular dominance and disparity coding in cat visual cortex. Visual Neuroscience, 1, 395–414. doi:10.1017/S0952523800004168.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lieberman, H. R. (1983). Computation of psychological thresholds using the probit technique. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 15, 446–448. doi:10.3758/BF03203681.CrossRef Lieberman, H. R. (1983). Computation of psychological thresholds using the probit technique. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 15, 446–448. doi:10.3758/BF03203681.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lieberman, H. R., & Pentland, A. P. (1982). Microcomputer-based estimation of psychophysical thresholds: The best PEST. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 14, 21–25. doi:10.3758/BF03202110.CrossRef Lieberman, H. R., & Pentland, A. P. (1982). Microcomputer-based estimation of psychophysical thresholds: The best PEST. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 14, 21–25. doi:10.3758/BF03202110.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide (2. Aufl.). Mahwah, New Jersey London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide (2. Aufl.). Mahwah, New Jersey London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Zurück zum Zitat Maertens, M., & Pollmann, S. (2005). fMRI reveals a common neural substrate of illusory and real contours in V1 after perceptual learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1553–1564. doi:10.1162/089892905774597209.PubMedCrossRef Maertens, M., & Pollmann, S. (2005). fMRI reveals a common neural substrate of illusory and real contours in V1 after perceptual learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1553–1564. doi:10.1162/089892905774597209.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Maisak, M. S., et al. (2013). A directional tuning map of Drosophila elementary motion detectors. Nature, 500, 212–216. doi:10.1038/nature12320.PubMedCrossRef Maisak, M. S., et al. (2013). A directional tuning map of Drosophila elementary motion detectors. Nature, 500, 212–216. doi:10.1038/nature12320.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York: Freeman and Company. Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York: Freeman and Company.
Zurück zum Zitat Marr, D., & Poggio, T. (1979). A computational theory of human stereo vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B 204, 301–328. doi:10.1098/rspb.1979.0029.PubMedCrossRef Marr, D., & Poggio, T. (1979). A computational theory of human stereo vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B 204, 301–328. doi:10.1098/rspb.1979.0029.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mashour, M. (1964). Psychophysical relations in the perception of velocity. Stockholm: Almquist and Wiksell. Mashour, M. (1964). Psychophysical relations in the perception of velocity. Stockholm: Almquist and Wiksell.
Zurück zum Zitat Meinecke, C., & Kehrer, L. (1994). Peripheral and foveal segmentation of angle textures. Perception and Psychophysics, 56, 326–334. doi:10.3758/BF03209766.PubMedCrossRef Meinecke, C., & Kehrer, L. (1994). Peripheral and foveal segmentation of angle textures. Perception and Psychophysics, 56, 326–334. doi:10.3758/BF03209766.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Merigan, W. H., & Maunsell, J. H. (1993). How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 369–403. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.002101.PubMedCrossRef Merigan, W. H., & Maunsell, J. H. (1993). How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 369–403. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.002101.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Metzger, W. (1932). Versuch einer gemeinsamen Theorie der Phänomene Fröhlichs und Hazeloffs und Kritik ihrer Verfahren zur Messung der Empfindungszeit. Psychologische Forschung, 16, 176–200. doi:10.1007/BF00409732.CrossRef Metzger, W. (1932). Versuch einer gemeinsamen Theorie der Phänomene Fröhlichs und Hazeloffs und Kritik ihrer Verfahren zur Messung der Empfindungszeit. Psychologische Forschung, 16, 176–200. doi:10.1007/BF00409732.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Metzger, W. (1966). Figurale Wahrnehmung. In W. Metzger (Hrsg.), Handbuch der Psychologie (Bd. 1, S. 693–744). Göttingen: Hogrefe. Metzger, W. (1966). Figurale Wahrnehmung. In W. Metzger (Hrsg.), Handbuch der Psychologie (Bd. 1, S. 693–744). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Zurück zum Zitat Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. G., & Macko, K. A. (1983). Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways. Trends in Neuroscience, 6, 414–417. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(83)90190-X.CrossRef Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. G., & Macko, K. A. (1983). Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways. Trends in Neuroscience, 6, 414–417. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(83)90190-X.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Spatial cueing and the relation between the accuracy of „where“ and „what“ decisions in visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 747–773. doi:10.1080/14640748908402392.CrossRef Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Spatial cueing and the relation between the accuracy of „where“ and „what“ decisions in visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 747–773. doi:10.1080/14640748908402392.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler, J. (1999a). How independent from action control is perception? An event-coding account for more equally-ranked crosstalks. In G. Aschersleben, & T. B. J. Müsseler (Hrsg.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (S. 121–147). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef Müsseler, J. (1999a). How independent from action control is perception? An event-coding account for more equally-ranked crosstalks. In G. Aschersleben, & T. B. J. Müsseler (Hrsg.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (S. 121–147). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler, J. (1999b). Perceiving and measuring of spatiotemporal events. In S. Jordan (Hrsg.), Modeling consciousness across the disciplines (S. 95–112). Lanham, Maryland: University Press of America, Inc. Müsseler, J. (1999b). Perceiving and measuring of spatiotemporal events. In S. Jordan (Hrsg.), Modeling consciousness across the disciplines (S. 95–112). Lanham, Maryland: University Press of America, Inc.
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler, J., & Aschersleben, G. (1998). Localizing the first position of a moving stimulus: The Fröhlich effect and an attention-shifting explanation. Perception and Psychophysics, 60, 683–695. doi:10.3758/BF03206055.PubMedCrossRef Müsseler, J., & Aschersleben, G. (1998). Localizing the first position of a moving stimulus: The Fröhlich effect and an attention-shifting explanation. Perception and Psychophysics, 60, 683–695. doi:10.3758/BF03206055.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler, J., & Kerzel, D. (2016). Mislocalizing the onset position of moving stimuli. In T. L. Hubbard (Hrsg.), Spatial Biases in Perception and Cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Müsseler, J., & Kerzel, D. (2016). Mislocalizing the onset position of moving stimuli. In T. L. Hubbard (Hrsg.), Spatial Biases in Perception and Cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler, J., & Tiggelbeck, J. (2013). The perceived onset position of a moving target: Effects of trial contexts are evoked by different attentional allocations. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 349–357. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0397-6.CrossRef Müsseler, J., & Tiggelbeck, J. (2013). The perceived onset position of a moving target: Effects of trial contexts are evoked by different attentional allocations. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 349–357. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0397-6.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler, J., & Van der Heijden, A. H. C. (2004). Two spatial maps contributing to perceived space. Evidence from a relative mislocalization. Visual Cognition, 11, 235–254. doi:10.1080/13506280344000338.CrossRef Müsseler, J., & Van der Heijden, A. H. C. (2004). Two spatial maps contributing to perceived space. Evidence from a relative mislocalization. Visual Cognition, 11, 235–254. doi:10.1080/13506280344000338.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler, J., Van der Heijden, A. H. C., Mahmud, S. H., Deubel, H., & Ertsey, S. (1999). Relative mislocalizations of briefly presented stimuli in the retinal periphery. Perception and Psychophysics, 61, 1646–1661. doi:10.3758/BF03213124.PubMedCrossRef Müsseler, J., Van der Heijden, A. H. C., Mahmud, S. H., Deubel, H., & Ertsey, S. (1999). Relative mislocalizations of briefly presented stimuli in the retinal periphery. Perception and Psychophysics, 61, 1646–1661. doi:10.3758/BF03213124.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler, J., Steininger, S., & Wühr, P. (2001). Can actions affect perceptual processing? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 137–154. doi:10.1080/02724980042000057.CrossRef Müsseler, J., Steininger, S., & Wühr, P. (2001). Can actions affect perceptual processing? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 137–154. doi:10.1080/02724980042000057.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Müsseler, J., Stork, S., & Kerzel, D. (2002). Comparing mislocalizations in movement direction: The Fröhlich effect, the flash-lag effect and representational momentum. Visual Cognition, 9, 120–138. doi:10.1080/13506280143000359.CrossRef Müsseler, J., Stork, S., & Kerzel, D. (2002). Comparing mislocalizations in movement direction: The Fröhlich effect, the flash-lag effect and representational momentum. Visual Cognition, 9, 120–138. doi:10.1080/13506280143000359.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nakayama, K. (1994). James J. Gibson: An appreciation. Psychological Review, 101, 329–335. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.2.329.PubMedCrossRef Nakayama, K. (1994). James J. Gibson: An appreciation. Psychological Review, 101, 329–335. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.2.329.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive Psychology. New York: Meredith Corporation. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive Psychology. New York: Meredith Corporation.
Zurück zum Zitat Nijhawan, R. (1994). Motion extrapolation in catching. Nature, 370, 256–257. doi:10.1038/370256b0.PubMedCrossRef Nijhawan, R. (1994). Motion extrapolation in catching. Nature, 370, 256–257. doi:10.1038/370256b0.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nothdurft, H. C. (1990). Texture discrimination by cells in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus. Experimental Brain Research, 82, 48–66. doi:10.1007/BF00230837.PubMedCrossRef Nothdurft, H. C. (1990). Texture discrimination by cells in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus. Experimental Brain Research, 82, 48–66. doi:10.1007/BF00230837.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Oudejans, R. R. D., Verheijen, R., Bakker, F. C., Gerrits, J. C., Steinbrückner, M., & Beek, P. J. (2000). Errors in judging ‘offside’ in football. Nature,, 404, 33.CrossRef Oudejans, R. R. D., Verheijen, R., Bakker, F. C., Gerrits, J. C., Steinbrückner, M., & Beek, P. J. (2000). Errors in judging ‘offside’ in football. Nature,, 404, 33.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Palmer, S. E. (1992). Common region: A new principle of perceptual grouping. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 436–447. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(92)90014-S.PubMedCrossRef Palmer, S. E. (1992). Common region: A new principle of perceptual grouping. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 436–447. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(92)90014-S.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Palmer, S. E. (1999). Vision science. Photons to phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Palmer, S. E. (1999). Vision science. Photons to phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Palmer, S., & Rock, I. (1994). Rethinking perceptual organization: The role of uniform connectedness. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 29–55. doi:10.3758/BF03200760.PubMedCrossRef Palmer, S., & Rock, I. (1994). Rethinking perceptual organization: The role of uniform connectedness. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 29–55. doi:10.3758/BF03200760.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pentland, A. P. (1980). The Best PEST, a maximum-likelihood parameter estimation procedure. Perception and Psychophysics, 28, 377–379.PubMedCrossRef Pentland, A. P. (1980). The Best PEST, a maximum-likelihood parameter estimation procedure. Perception and Psychophysics, 28, 377–379.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pizlo, Z. (1994). A theory of shape constancy based on perspective invariants. Vision Research, 34, 1637–1658. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)90123-6.PubMedCrossRef Pizlo, Z. (1994). A theory of shape constancy based on perspective invariants. Vision Research, 34, 1637–1658. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)90123-6.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Plessner, H., & Schallies, E. (2005). Judging the cross on rings: A matter of achieving shape sonstancy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1145–1156. doi:10.1002/acp.1136.CrossRef Plessner, H., & Schallies, E. (2005). Judging the cross on rings: A matter of achieving shape sonstancy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1145–1156. doi:10.1002/acp.1136.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pylyshyn, Z. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 341–365. doi:10.1017/S0140525X99002022.PubMed Pylyshyn, Z. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 341–365. doi:10.1017/S0140525X99002022.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Quinlan, P. T., & Wilton, R. N. (1998). Grouping by proximity or similarity? Competition between the Gestalt principles in vision. Perception, 27, 417–430. doi:10.1068/p270417.PubMedCrossRef Quinlan, P. T., & Wilton, R. N. (1998). Grouping by proximity or similarity? Competition between the Gestalt principles in vision. Perception, 27, 417–430. doi:10.1068/p270417.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rock, I., Nijhawan, R., Palmer, S., & Tudor, L. (1992). Grouping based on phenomenal similarity of achromatic color. Perception, 21, 779–789. doi:10.1068/p210779.PubMedCrossRef Rock, I., Nijhawan, R., Palmer, S., & Tudor, L. (1992). Grouping based on phenomenal similarity of achromatic color. Perception, 21, 779–789. doi:10.1068/p210779.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rodgers, N. (1999). Unglaubliche optische Illusionen. Augsburg: Weltbild. Rodgers, N. (1999). Unglaubliche optische Illusionen. Augsburg: Weltbild.
Zurück zum Zitat Roelfsma, P. R., & Singer, W. (1998). Detecting connectedness. Cerebral Cortex, 85, 385–396. doi:10.1093/cercor/8.5.385.CrossRef Roelfsma, P. R., & Singer, W. (1998). Detecting connectedness. Cerebral Cortex, 85, 385–396. doi:10.1093/cercor/8.5.385.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rossetti, Y., & Pisella, L. (2002). Several ‘vision for action’ systems: A guide to dissociating and integrating dorsal and ventral functions. In W. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Hrsg.), Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (S. 62–119). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Rossetti, Y., & Pisella, L. (2002). Several ‘vision for action’ systems: A guide to dissociating and integrating dorsal and ventral functions. In W. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Hrsg.), Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (S. 62–119). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Rubin, E. (1921/2001). Figure and ground. In S. Yantis (Ed.), Visual perception (pp. 225–229; Auszug aus Rubin, E. (1921). Visuell wahrgenommene Figuren. Kopenhagen: Gyldendalske Boghandel). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. Rubin, E. (1921/2001). Figure and ground. In S. Yantis (Ed.), Visual perception (pp. 225–229; Auszug aus Rubin, E. (1921). Visuell wahrgenommene Figuren. Kopenhagen: Gyldendalske Boghandel). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Sagi, D. (1995). The psychophysics of texture segmentation. In T. V. Papathomas, & C. Chubb (Hrsg.), Early vision and beyond (S. 69–78). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Sagi, D. (1995). The psychophysics of texture segmentation. In T. V. Papathomas, & C. Chubb (Hrsg.), Early vision and beyond (S. 69–78). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Schiller, P. H., Logothetis, N. K., & Charles, E. R. (1990). Functions of the colour-opponent and broad-band channels of the visual system. Nature, 343, 68–70. doi:10.1038/343068a0.PubMedCrossRef Schiller, P. H., Logothetis, N. K., & Charles, E. R. (1990). Functions of the colour-opponent and broad-band channels of the visual system. Nature, 343, 68–70. doi:10.1038/343068a0.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schrauf, M., Lingelbach, B., & Wist, E. R. (1997). The scintillating grid illusion. Vision Research, 37, 1033–1038. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00255-6.PubMedCrossRef Schrauf, M., Lingelbach, B., & Wist, E. R. (1997). The scintillating grid illusion. Vision Research, 37, 1033–1038. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00255-6.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Seckel, A. (2014). Optische Illusionen: Sie werden Ihren Augen nicht trauen! Fränkisch-Crumbach: Edition XXL Verlag. Seckel, A. (2014). Optische Illusionen: Sie werden Ihren Augen nicht trauen! Fränkisch-Crumbach: Edition XXL Verlag.
Zurück zum Zitat Selfridge, O. G. (1959). Pandemonium: A Paradigm for learning. Proceedings of Mechanisation of Thought Processes, Bd. 1, S. 513–530). London: Her Majesty’s Stationary Office. Selfridge, O. G. (1959). Pandemonium: A Paradigm for learning. Proceedings of Mechanisation of Thought Processes, Bd. 1, S. 513–530). London: Her Majesty’s Stationary Office.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaw, R. E., Flascher, O. M., & Mace, W. M. (1995). Dimensions of event perception. In W. Prinz, & B. Bridgeman (Hrsg.), Perception Handbook of perception and action, (Bd. 1, S. 345–395). London, UK: Academic Press.CrossRef Shaw, R. E., Flascher, O. M., & Mace, W. M. (1995). Dimensions of event perception. In W. Prinz, & B. Bridgeman (Hrsg.), Perception Handbook of perception and action, (Bd. 1, S. 345–395). London, UK: Academic Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shen, J., & Reingold, E. M. (2001). Visual search asymmetry: The influence of stimulus familiarity and low-level features. Perception and Psychophysics, 63, 464–475. doi:10.3758/BF03194413.PubMedCrossRef Shen, J., & Reingold, E. M. (2001). Visual search asymmetry: The influence of stimulus familiarity and low-level features. Perception and Psychophysics, 63, 464–475. doi:10.3758/BF03194413.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Singer, W. (1994). The organization of sensory motor representations in the Neocortex: A hypothesis based on temporal binding. In C. Umiltà, & M. Moscovitch (Hrsg.), Attention and Performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (S. 77–107). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Singer, W. (1994). The organization of sensory motor representations in the Neocortex: A hypothesis based on temporal binding. In C. Umiltà, & M. Moscovitch (Hrsg.), Attention and Performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (S. 77–107). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Singer, W., Engel, A. K., Kreiter, A. K., Munk, M. H. J., Neuenschwander, S., & Roelfsema, P. R. (1997). Neuronal assemblies: necessity, signature and detectability. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 252–261. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01079-6.PubMedCrossRef Singer, W., Engel, A. K., Kreiter, A. K., Munk, M. H. J., Neuenschwander, S., & Roelfsema, P. R. (1997). Neuronal assemblies: necessity, signature and detectability. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 252–261. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01079-6.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Skavenski, A. A. (1990). Eye movement and visual localization of objects in space. In E. Kowler (Hrsg.), Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes (S. 263–287). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Skavenski, A. A. (1990). Eye movement and visual localization of objects in space. In E. Kowler (Hrsg.), Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes (S. 263–287). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Zurück zum Zitat Spillmann, L. (1994). The Hermann grid illusion: A tool for studying human perceptive field organization. Perception, 23, 691–708. doi:10.1068/p230691.PubMedCrossRef Spillmann, L. (1994). The Hermann grid illusion: A tool for studying human perceptive field organization. Perception, 23, 691–708. doi:10.1068/p230691.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64, 153–181. doi:10.1037/h0046162.PubMedCrossRef Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64, 153–181. doi:10.1037/h0046162.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stevens, S. S. (1975). Psychophysics. New York: Wiley and Sons. Stevens, S. S. (1975). Psychophysics. New York: Wiley and Sons.
Zurück zum Zitat Swets, J. A., Tanner, W. P., & Birdsall, T. G. (1961). Decision processes in perception. Psychological Review, 68, 301–340. doi:10.1037/h0040547.PubMedCrossRef Swets, J. A., Tanner, W. P., & Birdsall, T. G. (1961). Decision processes in perception. Psychological Review, 68, 301–340. doi:10.1037/h0040547.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tanaka, K. (1996). Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 19, 109–139. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.19.1.109.PubMedCrossRef Tanaka, K. (1996). Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 19, 109–139. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.19.1.109.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tessier-Lavigne, M., & Gouras, P. (1996). Farbe. In E. R. Kandel, & J. H. S. T. H. Jessell (Hrsg.), Neurowissenschaften. Eine Einführung (S. 459–475). Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag. Tessier-Lavigne, M., & Gouras, P. (1996). Farbe. In E. R. Kandel, & J. H. S. T. H. Jessell (Hrsg.), Neurowissenschaften. Eine Einführung (S. 459–475). Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.
Zurück zum Zitat Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5.PubMedCrossRef Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tsal, Y., & Lavie, N. (1988). Attending to color and shape: the special role of location in selective visual processing. Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 15–21. doi:10.3758/BF03207469.PubMedCrossRef Tsal, Y., & Lavie, N. (1988). Attending to color and shape: the special role of location in selective visual processing. Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 15–21. doi:10.3758/BF03207469.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tyler, C. W. (1990). A stereoscopic view of visual processing streams. Vision Research, 30, 1877–1895. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(90)90165-H.PubMedCrossRef Tyler, C. W. (1990). A stereoscopic view of visual processing streams. Vision Research, 30, 1877–1895. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(90)90165-H.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, & M. A. G. R. J. Mansfield (Hrsg.), Analysis of visual behavior (S. 549–580). Cambridge: MIT Press. Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, & M. A. G. R. J. Mansfield (Hrsg.), Analysis of visual behavior (S. 549–580). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Heijden, A. H. C. (1992). Selective attention in vision. London: Routledge. Van der Heijden, A. H. C. (1992). Selective attention in vision. London: Routledge.
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Heijden, A. H. C. (2004). Attention in Vision – Perception, Communication, and Action. London: Routledge. Van der Heijden, A. H. C. (2004). Attention in Vision – Perception, Communication, and Action. London: Routledge.
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Heijden, A. H. C., Wolters, G., & Brouwer, R. F. T. (1995). Response dependency and processing dependency of line orientation and position in a single-item task. Psychological Research, 58, 19–30. doi:10.1007/BF00447086.CrossRef Van der Heijden, A. H. C., Wolters, G., & Brouwer, R. F. T. (1995). Response dependency and processing dependency of line orientation and position in a single-item task. Psychological Research, 58, 19–30. doi:10.1007/BF00447086.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Heijden, A. H. C., Kurvink, A. G., De Lange, L., De Leeuw, F., & Van der Geest, J. N. (1996). Attending to color with proper fixation. Perception and Psychophysics, 58, 1224–1237. doi:10.3758/BF03207555.PubMedCrossRef Van der Heijden, A. H. C., Kurvink, A. G., De Lange, L., De Leeuw, F., & Van der Geest, J. N. (1996). Attending to color with proper fixation. Perception and Psychophysics, 58, 1224–1237. doi:10.3758/BF03207555.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Heijden, A. H. C., Müsseler, J., & Bridgeman, B. (1999). On the perception of position. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (Hrsg.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (S. 19–37). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef Van der Heijden, A. H. C., Müsseler, J., & Bridgeman, B. (1999). On the perception of position. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (Hrsg.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (S. 19–37). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van Essen, D. C., & DeYoe, E. A. (1995). Concurrent processing. In I. M. S. Gazzaniga (Hrsg.), The cognitive neuroscience (S. 383–400). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Van Essen, D. C., & DeYoe, E. A. (1995). Concurrent processing. In I. M. S. Gazzaniga (Hrsg.), The cognitive neuroscience (S. 383–400). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Verri, A., Straforini, M., & Torre, V. (1992). Computational aspects of motion perception in natural and artificial visionsystems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 337(1282), 429–443. doi:10.1098/rstb.1992.0119.CrossRef Verri, A., Straforini, M., & Torre, V. (1992). Computational aspects of motion perception in natural and artificial visionsystems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 337(1282), 429–443. doi:10.1098/rstb.1992.0119.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wallach, H. (1948). Brightness constancy and the nature of achromatic colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 310–324. doi:10.1037/h0053804.PubMedCrossRef Wallach, H. (1948). Brightness constancy and the nature of achromatic colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 310–324. doi:10.1037/h0053804.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wallach, H. (1959). The perception of motion. Scientific American, 201, 107–116. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0759-56.CrossRef Wallach, H. (1959). The perception of motion. Scientific American, 201, 107–116. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0759-56.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Walsh, V., & Kulikowski, J. (Hrsg.). (1998). Perceptual constancy: Why things look as they do. New York: Cambridge University Press. Walsh, V., & Kulikowski, J. (Hrsg.). (1998). Perceptual constancy: Why things look as they do. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Wertheimer, M. (1912). Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 61, 262–265. Wertheimer, M. (1912). Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 61, 262–265.
Zurück zum Zitat Wertheimer, M. (1923). Untersuchungen zur Lehre der Gestalt, II. Psychologische Forschung, 5, 301–350. doi:10.1007/BF00410640.CrossRef Wertheimer, M. (1923). Untersuchungen zur Lehre der Gestalt, II. Psychologische Forschung, 5, 301–350. doi:10.1007/BF00410640.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Westheimer, G. (1981). Visual hyperacuity. Progress in Sensory Physiology (S. 1–30). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef Westheimer, G. (1981). Visual hyperacuity. Progress in Sensory Physiology (S. 1–30). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Whitney, D., Murakami, I., & Cavanagh, P. (2000). Illusory spatial offset of a flash relative to a moving stimulus is caused by differential latencies for moving and flashed stimuli. Vision Research, 40, 137–149. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00166-2.PubMedCrossRef Whitney, D., Murakami, I., & Cavanagh, P. (2000). Illusory spatial offset of a flash relative to a moving stimulus is caused by differential latencies for moving and flashed stimuli. Vision Research, 40, 137–149. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00166-2.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wirtz, M. (2001). Der Einfluss der Kantenkontrastverarbeitung auf die wahrgenommene Helligkeit angrenzender Flächen. München: Herbter Utz Verlag. Wirtz, M. (2001). Der Einfluss der Kantenkontrastverarbeitung auf die wahrgenommene Helligkeit angrenzender Flächen. München: Herbter Utz Verlag.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolff, P. (1999). Space perception and the intention of action. In G. Aschersleben, & T. B. J. Müsseler (Hrsg.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (S. 43–63). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef Wolff, P. (1999). Space perception and the intention of action. In G. Aschersleben, & T. B. J. Müsseler (Hrsg.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (S. 43–63). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Yantis, S. (Hrsg.). (2001). Visual perception. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. Yantis, S. (Hrsg.). (2001). Visual perception. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Zihl, J., Cramon, D. von, & Mai, N. (1983). Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral damage. Brain, 106, 313–340. doi:10.1093/brain/106.2.313.PubMedCrossRef Zihl, J., Cramon, D. von, & Mai, N. (1983). Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral damage. Brain, 106, 313–340. doi:10.1093/brain/106.2.313.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zeki, S., Watson, J. D. G., Lueck, C. J., Firston, K. J., Kennard, C., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1991). A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 641–649.PubMed Zeki, S., Watson, J. D. G., Lueck, C. J., Firston, K. J., Kennard, C., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1991). A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 641–649.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Zihl, J., Cramon, D. von, Mai, N., & Schmid, C. (1991). Disturbance of movement vision after bilateral posterior brain damage. Brain, 114, 2235–2252. doi:10.1093/brain/114.5.2235.PubMedCrossRef Zihl, J., Cramon, D. von, Mai, N., & Schmid, C. (1991). Disturbance of movement vision after bilateral posterior brain damage. Brain, 114, 2235–2252. doi:10.1093/brain/114.5.2235.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Visuelle Informationsverarbeitung
verfasst von
Prof. Dr. Jochen Müsseler
Copyright-Jahr
2017
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-53898-8_2

Premium Partner