Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Happiness Studies 4/2023

Open Access 03.04.2023 | Research Paper

A Public, Open, and Independently-Curated Database of Happiness Coefficients

verfasst von: C. P. Barrington-Leigh, Katja Lemermeyer

Erschienen in: Journal of Happiness Studies | Ausgabe 4/2023

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

We present a nascent database of happiness coefficients. This is a synthesis of evidence on the size of improvements to human life experience that can be expected from changing objective, policy-amenable circumstances. The wealth of data on people’s self-reported satisfaction with life in a wide variety of circumstances, from around the world, including respondents undergoing a diversity of changes and life events and subject to a variety of public policies and policy changes, has provided a rich base of knowledge about what makes life good. This growing research literature has in recent years been met with interest from central governments looking for accountable but more human-centred approaches to measuring progress, as well as for communicating objectives, making policy, and allocating resources. Meanwhile, frameworks for benefit-cost accounting using inference from life satisfaction data have been devised. In some cases central government finance departments and treasuries are incorporating this approach into their formal methodology for budgeting. The body of causal inference about these effects is still somewhat diffuse. Collating, reviewing, and synthesizing such evidence should be led initially by academia and ultimately by a broad academic, civil society, and government collaboration. We report on the assembly of a database of summary estimates for Canada, supplemented where needed by evidence from around the world. The categorized domains of individual experience and circumstances include Education, Environment, Work, Finances, Health, Social Capital, and Crime. The paper also explains the context for and limitations of the use of a database of happiness coefficients.
Hinweise

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

1 Introduction: Precedent Databases

As governments strive to establish new and updated frameworks for the evaluation and planning of programs, policies, and budgets based on modern evidence about human well-being, some new institutions will be needed (Barrington-Leigh, 2021). This paper (1) suggests some principles for the curation of growing knowledge about what makes for a good life, and happy societies; (2) provides a fledgling sample of what a database of such research findings might look like; and (3) articulates some important limitations on how such knowledge can be used in policy making.
Throughout this paper, the term “happiness” can be taken as a short hand to mean the set of subjective wellbeing measures that are used to gauge overall quality of life, most prominent and important among them being respondents’ own numerical evaluation of their level of satisfaction with life (SWL), obtained through a single survey question. The focus on response data to this one specific subgroup of subjective well-being questions, known as “evaluative” subjective well-being or as “cognitive evaluations of life,” is motivated by extensive evidence that it best captures the impacts of enduring lived circumstances, while subjective well-being questions focused more on affective states are better suited to capture day-to-day influences (e.g., Abdel-Khalek, 2006; Helliwell et al., 2022). It is also the measure recommended to government statistical agencies for use as an overarching measure of well-being (OECD, 2013; Stone & Christopher, 2014).
Reviews of econometric studies of happiness have in several instances compiled summary effect sizes into tabular form, in which the existing evidence on several different influences on life satisfaction are brought together (Clark et al., 2019, online Annexes 2–5). A more comprehensive but less synthetic approach is embodied as part of the World Data Base of Happiness (Veenhoven, n.d.). Its “Correlational Findings” section reports estimates of effects on happiness from a vast number of studies.
Frijters et al. (2020) describe a process by which the UK might maintain an authoritative list of the best available estimates for any given influence on wellbeing. Frijters et al.’s description may represent an overly-frequentist conception of filtering and aggregating evidence, but they emphasize the importance of moving towards transparency in however the updating of the database is carried out. Barrington-Leigh (2021) similarly advocates for a process to debate and distill knowledge about the relationship between policy-influenced variables and human experience, in an accountable and ongoing process, but suggests that this be led initially by the analytic community, rather than initially by government, with a transition towards more independence over time.

2 Principles for Curation

In what follows, we dub the database of such knowledge a Database of Happiness Coefficients (DoHC) and, for the purposes of discussion, the public body tasked with curating it a Wellbeing Knowledge Centre (WKC). We propose the following principles for a WKC and DoHC to support policy making by government:
1.
The DoHC should be curated independently or at arms-length from government. This is to ensure that all findings wll be made available to the public as well as to government agencies. While the knowledge embodied in the DoHC will never be sufficient to dictate policies (see Sect. 4), it must be available to the public and to public and private organizations in order to help to push government to adopt a more evidence- and human- oriented policy making approach.
 
2.
The WKC must strive for maximum transparency of its methods, including the criteria for selection and integration of studies. Collating, reviewing, and synthesizing evidence for the DoHC should be a collaborative undertaking, with engagement from all interested stakeholders. Initially, this task should likely be led by academia but ultimately it should become a broad academic, civil society, and government collaboration. This will ensure that the evidence remains (1) robust, (2) inclusive of evidence, such as government program trials, which is high quality but may not be published, and (3) likely to be both used and usable by interested parties. A possible productive service for the WKC would be to host an ongoing open review process of all research sources used to build the DoHC.
 
3.
The WKC must always embrace an openness to revision of the database. Core to the DoHC are evaluations of the degree of confidence in causal inferences behind each coefficient. Future evidence will continuously revise and deepen the DoHC.
 
4.
The DoHC should be designed to inform calculations about the expected distribution of wellbeing. Most frequentist statistical models in use in this field focus on estimating mean values of wellbeing, or use strong distributional assumptions, but these may ultimately prove inadequate to inform policy choices, which will be based on the full predicted distributions of wellbeing outcomes. That is, policy makers will want to consider both the univariate distribution of (for instance) SWL, as well as its variation along standard dimensions of disadvantage, oppression, and inequality.
 
5.
The DoHC should target content to support the needs of planners and decision makers. Explanatory variables (predictive factors) in academic models are often chosen based on (a) their hypothesized importance in accounting for variance in happiness, or (b) simply on their availability, or (c) on being able to show or argue that their variation constitutes or contains a natural experiment of some kind. In order to be useful to decision makers and community planners, estimates will instead need increasingly to focus on the effects caused by objective, policy-amenable outcomes. One may therefore expect initially many knowledge gaps in the DoHC. The WKC may need to help direct resources to fill those gaps. In order to be accessible and useful, evidence in the DoHC also needs to be available in a format or formats tailored to the needs and capacities of relevant analysts and policy makers. It will need to be effectively communicated to ensure both awareness and timely attention.
 
6.
The DoHC should be constructed so as to allow for hierarchically-sourced evidence and be able to privilege locally-contextualized evidence. A national WKC will incorporate evidence from around the world and liaise with other national or international curators of DoHCs, or possibly to lead in the curation of an international one. In any case, locally-contextualized evidence should be given appropriate priority, and at all geographic scales. A municipal or community government will need to lean heavily on evidence about wellbeing gathered from beyond its jurisdiction, but at the same time will want to emphasize local experience.
 
In practice, large government departments may inevitably maintain their own version of the DoHC internally, but it is expected that internal government studies and experience will eventually make it into the public domain, so that novel information should ultimately all flow into the public DoHC.
The WKC will most likely need to commission studies to summarize the knowledge in a given field, and incorporate the synthesized findings into the DoHC. The What Works Centre for Wellbeing in the U.K. is already playing this role of commissioning reviews (e.g., What Works Centre for Wellbeing, 2018).

3 Seed DoHC for Canada

In the interest of seeding an effort of building a DoHC for Canada, and in order to communicate the concept, we report the construction of a small DoHC.

3.1 Methods

Briefly, the following procedure was carried out to arrive at our database entries. First, a search of EconLit, EconPapers, Scopus, and JSTOR for publications in economics and psychology led to a set of 189 academic articles and working papers related to life satisfaction in Canada.
Secondly, these papers were retrieved and sorted by topic. Features such as survey data used, sample size, age of respondents, geographic scope, temporality (cross-section or longitudinal), and the subjective wellbeing measure in use were all tagged.
Thirdly, studies with large sample sizes, relevant SWB measures, nationwide scope and/or longitudinal data were preferentially chosen. Within each, we identified estimates derived through well-defined methodologies and evaluated the confidence in their effect and causality. These features were recorded in the database, along with any free-form comments or clarifications.
Our database is similar in intent to that of the What Works Centre for Wellbeing (2018), and different from that of the World Database of Happiness (Veenhoven, n.d.), in that it aims to synthesize a literature and interpret the relevance, confidence, and causal identification of available studies rather than to comprehensively enumerate them all. This process will always require judgment, but (Bayesian) statistical procedures needed to achieve the principles described above, especially the sixth, in a reproducible way will still need to be developed.

3.2 Findings

Our database is available online at https://​lifesatisfaction​.​ca/​dohc and included in full (as at the time of writing) at the end of this manuscript. Table 1 shows a few sample values from the database, which also includes commentary on the persistence of effects over time, the degree of confidence in effect and causality, the data source and type, and of course the relevant citation(s).
Table 1
Sample entries in the Canadian DoHC from several different domains
Domain
Change
Effect on 0–10 SWL
Education
Extra compulsory year
− 0.03 (± 0.098)
Environment
\(\uparrow\)SO\(_2\) by 10 \(\upmu\)g m\(^{-3}\)
− 0.04 (±.04)
Work
Overqualification
− 0.280 (±.049)
Finances
doubling of HH income
+ 0.16 (±.196)
Health
smoking daily\(\rightarrow\)never
+ 0.12 (±.04)
Social Capital
Partnered\(\rightarrow\)separated
− 0.4 (± 0.14)
Crime
Victim of violent crime
− 0.396
See Table 2 at the end of the paper for fuller details

4 The Role of Happiness Policy in Context

In any discussion of the life satisfaction approach to benefit-cost accounting, it is important to keep the context in mind. There is a lot that any DoHC or wellbeing policy approach will never be able to do, and DoHCs do not have the potential to diminish policy making towards a deterministic or technocratic exercise. This section describes three important limitations (discussed in more detail in Barrington-Leigh 2021) to what can be expected from a DoHC.

4.1 Distributions

First, as mentioned above, the knowledge base around predicting policy effects on wellbeing should in principle be designed to predict distributions of outcomes, not just averages. Having a good understanding of wellbeing impacts means one can disaggregate the overall effects of a given policy or budget based on different demographic groups or subpopulations and, importantly, intersectional groups. Many governments, when carrying out evaluations or projections, already disaggregate outcomes in this way. Using a new or more encompassing measure of wellbeing as an objective does not change the need nor challenge of understanding distributional outcomes.
Moreover, those distributional outcomes are fundamental to decision making. While the early literature on cost/benefit accounting for policy-making (e.g., Happiness Research Institute, 2020; Frijters and Krekel, 2021) emphasizes scalar objectives and descision criteria, in reality decision makers are sensitive to non-scalar considerations. For instance, Fig. 1a, b show hypothetical distributions of current and projected future life satisfaction. The prospective policy appears to increase wellbeing from 6.6 to 7.3, according to its mean, yet the distribution shows that some people are worse off afterwards than before. Panel (c) disaggregates the anticipated outcome into a demographic subgroup (shown in orange) and the rest of the population (shown in blue). The relative lack of thriving of the subgroup may be a considerable concern for policy makers for ethical (equity) or political reasons. In any case, nothing about a life satisfaction approach nor the information in a DoHC will resolve the question of how to value different parts of a distribution in coming to an overall decision. These kinds of considerations do not happen automatically with a wellbeing approach, just as they do not happen automatically when using traditional welfare measures like family income.

4.2 Dynamics

There is a second reason that a DoHC does not act as a policy oracle. Policy makers may disagree about whether reducing a given disparity is best carried out through strong government intervention and redistribution, or more through removing barriers and allowing for people to change their own situation. However, this question is not just about ethics and principle, but also about the dynamics of how people behave and invest over their life course, and indeed how all kinds of possible and typical government investments pay off over time. Those are questions to which a wellbeing approach assumes you already know the answer. That is, the DoHC is likely to specialize, especially early on, in answering the question, “Given a set of objective conditions at some (future) point in time, how happy would someone be?” In order to project the outcome of a policy change or budget allocation today, one will need to predict future objective conditions driven by the policy change. This information is all outside of the DoHC’s contribution (for more explanation, see Barrington-Leigh, 2021, 2022). If anything, though, the policy synergies made possible by having an overarching, well-understood measure of wellbeing may make it much more desirable and valuable for governments to have sophisticated and detailed models of the return to human and non-human investments over the life course.

4.3 Precautionary Approach

Despite the limitations above, the most ambitious and attractive promise of a wellbeing approach is that it offers a way to add up all the effects of taxation, legislation, and expenditure, along with extant conditions, to come up with a reasonable prediction of the distribution of outcomes for a prospective policy. This system, which boasts accountability to measurable outcomes and a growing evidence base, can provide cost/benefit or cost effectiveness guidance to a decision maker who has a way to handle distributional questions.
However, there is another dimension in which this vision has its limits: one cannot feasibly apply the wellbeing approach to all questions about future public investments. In particular, when considering questions about some investments with far-future payoffs, the uncertainty in predictions of objective outcomes will lead to a large amount of uncertainty about the implications for future human wellbeing. This uncertainty can overwhelm any decision-making clarity for decisions about alternative uses and benefits of a resource in the short term. That is, for long-run, unfamiliar, unpredictable, complex, and uncertain dynamics, the calculations described in the previous sections may not provide precise enough answers for making decisions in the same way that shorter-run decisions can be made. They will not always be able, therefore, to direct us when making choices between short-term outcomes and long-run outcomes.
This limitation is, again, nothing to do with switching to a more evidence-informed metric for human wellbeing. It is instead an existing challenge that is unchanged by the availability of a DoHC except in that it comes into sharper focus. When one has a more explicit measure of human wellbeing, the question of whether policy is simply meant to maximise it is starker than when pursuing vague, proxy objectives like economic growth, which no one would argue is a singular goal of optimal policy. The implication of this limitation is that some other principle, i.e., beyond wellbeing maximisation, is needed to make long-run decisions whose ramifications are particularly speculative or far-off. Barrington-Leigh (2021, section 6.1) again describes the alternative, or solution, in more detail, and associates these long-run quandaries with the idea of sustainability. A “precautionary approach” is typical language for how to handle such uncertainty when the costs and benefits for human wellbeing are not sufficiently understood or precise.

5 Conclusion

The availability of a DoHC with sufficient coverage and precision to be useful for informing government decision-making has become an imminent reality. The UK Treasury (2021) already has explicit guidance in place for this kind of quantitative evaluation. Canada’s new Quality of Life framework (Department of Finance, 2021) is perfectly suited to benefit from it also. On the way there, however, are significant capacity gaps and institutional transitions. A close relationship with academic researchers will be necessary in the beginning to construct this important database of human knowledge. The nascent DoHC in this paper may serve as an example for researchers and government agencies to begin thinking about how to shape, organize, and curate such information in an open and transparent and geographically hierarchical way.
As this idea permeates government agencies, a few cautions or points of advice are in order, and described above. To reiterate, (1) quantitative wellbeing approaches do not release governments from the duty of judging questions of distribution and equity; nor do they diminish the role of politics and debate in this task; (2) a DoHC does not predict the future; it only tells us how a given future may map onto experienced wellbeing; great efforts are needed in bolstering governments’ abilities to model returns to investments, in particular investments in people which bear fruit throughout the life course; and (3) many questions of long-run sustainability cannot be sufficiently handled through quantitative optimization of wellbeing and should instead be debated and settled using an alternative framing principle, such as the goal of more arbitrary conservation.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful for the suggestions by one anonymous reviewer. This work was supported by Canada’s Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) grant 435-2016-0531.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge

Appendix

Table 2
Table 2
Entries in the DoHC, available in a sortable, downloadable, and updated form are at lifesatisfaction​.​ca/​dohc
Domain
Category
Change
Effect on 0–10 life satisfaction
Dynamics
Confidence in effect and causality
Source, Country, and Comments
Crime
Fear
A doubling fear of crime
Approx \(-\) 0.30
Unknown
Medium. Panel data-based, often replicated, but drivers of fear not exogenous
Hanslmaier (2013), “derived from the relative effect of fera of crime versus effect from unemployment in a log-odds setting” (note on this reference in Frijters handbook). Nationwide representative study on victimization and crime-related issues, 2010 (Panel; DEU). Derived from relative effect of fear of crime versus effect from unemployment in a log-odds setting
Crime
Violent crime
Victim of violent crime
\(-\) 0.396
Effect largely in first year (only statistically significant in first year)
High but specific: effects are for unanticipated events that were recorded
Johnston et al. (2018), Table 3 (?) Effect of \(-\) 0.398 for females and − .300 for males. HILDA 2002–12 (Panel; AUS)
Education
Duration
Extra year of compulsory education
\(-\) 0.03 (± 0.098) converted from 1–7 to 0–10 LS
Persistent effects
High for UK; since effect found from 1972 UK compulsory school changes. Marginal result also found in other Western countries
Clark and Jung (2017), Page 11, paragraph 1 (based on Table 3). BHPS 1996–2008 (Panel; GBR)
Environment
Air pollution
Increase of 1-day SO\(_2\) level by 10 \(\upmu\)g m\(^{-3}\) (equivalent to 3.9 ppb)
\(-\) 0.02 (±  0.02) on 5-point LS
Temporary effect
Effect robust in cross-sectional data; includes high-resolution geographic fixed effects
Barrington-Leigh and Behzadnejad (2017a), In text, bottom of page 16 of paper. CCHS 2005–11 (Cross-sectional; CAN)
Environment
Air pollution
Increase of average PM10 level by 10 \(\mu\)g m\(^{-3}\) (equivalent to 3.9 ppb)
0.014 on a 3-point happiness scale
Unknown
Medium to high; effects of air pollution significantly exogenous for single individual
Levinson (2012), Results section paragraph 1. GSS (USA) 1984–96 (Cross-sectional; USA)
Environment
Air pollution
Increase of average SO\(_2\) level by 10 \(\upmu\)g m\(^{-3}\) (equivalent to 3.9 ppb)
\(-\) 0.08
Unknown
High; effects driven by unanticipated changes in power plant emissions due to policy
Luechinger (2009), Table 4, column II (IV estimate). GSOEP 1983–2011 (Panel; DEU)
Environment
Land use
Construction of wind turbine within 4 km around household
\(-\) 0.1405 (± 0.0782)
Seems temporary; effect disappears after 5 years
High; wind turbine construction exogenous for household in surroundings, difference-in-differences with treatment at multiple points in time
Krekel and Zerrahn (2017), Table 2, column 1. GSOEP 2000–2012 (Panel; DEU)
Environment
Land use
Increase of 1 hectare of greenspace within 1 km of household
+ 0.0031 converted from 1–7 to 0–10 LS
Seems permanent
Medium to high; panel data-based set but no clearcut exogenous variation
White et al. (2013), 0.0020 in Table 2, Column 5. BHPS 1991–2008 (Panel; GBR). Cited by / taken from DOHC in Frijters and Krekel...?
Environment
Land use
Increase of 1 hectare of greenspace within 1 km of household
+ 0.0066 (±  0.0049)
Seems permanent
Medium to high; panel data-based set but no clearcut exogenous variation; similar results by studies in the UK
Krekel et al. (2016), Table B.2. GSOEP 2000–2012 (Panel; DEU). Effects strongest for older residents
Environment
Land use
Increase of 1 hectare of vacant land (abandoned areas) within 1 km of household
\(-\) 0.0395 (±  0.0002)
Unknown
Medium; panel data-based but no clearcut exogenous variation
Krekel et al. (2016), Table B.2. GSOEP 2000–2012 (Panel; DEU). Effects strongest for older residents
Environment
Weather
Daily rainfall of 6 mm above average
\(-\) 0.008 (±  0.0012) on 5-point LS
Temporary effect
Effect is statistically significant and robust in cross-sectional dataset, but not in panel dataset
Barrington-Leigh and Behzadnejad (2017b), Table 2, Columns 7 and 8. CCHS 2005–11, NPHS 2004–10 (Cross-sectional and panel; CAN). Women and individuals with poor health condition are more affected
Finances
Financial satisfaction
High financial stress (self-rated)
\(-\) 0.864 (± 0.086)
Unknown
Cross-sectional data, considering the possibility of an indirect effect of income through financial stress uncovers a strong effect of financial stress on life satisfaction, but an effect not clearly linked to income
Brzozowski and Visano (2020), Table 2, Column 2. GSS 19–24 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Measurement includes those who report 3 or higher on a 5-point stress scale and also choose “finances” as their primary source of stress
Finances
Income
Doubling of household income
+ 0.16 (±  0.196)
Persistent effects with elation peak
High. Effect found in panels, cross-sections, and shock-related (lotteries)
Flèche et al. (2019), Table 2.1. BCS70 (Panel; GBR). Height disputed and income measurement problematic
Finances
Income
Doubling of household income
+ 0.5
Persistent effects with elation peak
High. Effect found in panels, cross-sections, and shock-related (lotteries)
Frijters et al. (2004), Table 2. GSOEP 1991–2001 (Panel; DEU)
Finances
Income
Increase in difference between own log income and log income of a provincial reference group
+ 0.194 (±  0.135)
Unknown
Medium. Panel data, significant negative effect as found in other Canadian literature
Latif (2016), Table 5, Column 2. NPHS 1994–2009 (Panel; CAN). Reference group contains all individuals with a similar education level that are inside the same age bracket and residing in the same province
Finances
Prosocial spending
Donated to charity in the past month
+ 0.27(± 0.039) on 11-point Cantril ladder
Unknown
Cross-sectional data, relies on correlational analysis, supported by limited experimental data
Aknin et al. (2013), GWP 2006–08 (Cross-sectional and panel; WLD)
Finances
Prosocial spending
Donated to charity in the past month
+ 0.28 (± 0.047) on 11-point Cantril ladder
Unknown
Cross-sectional data, relies on correlational analysis, supported by limited experimental data
Aknin et al. (2013), GWP 2006–08 (Cross-sectional and panel; USA, CAN, AUS, NZL). Region-specific coefficient using survey results from US, Canada, Australia, NZ
Health
  
+ 0.24 (± \(-\) 0.03)
Effect lasts while treatment lasts
Medium. Fixed-effect estimates consistent with small RCTs and public health campaign results, but magnitude very unclear
Mujcic and Oswald (2016), Table 2, column 1 and 2; in text near beginning of page 3. HILDA 2007, 2009 (Panel; AUS)
Health
Mental health
From depression to full mental health
+ 0.71
Permanent, little evidence of a peak
High as found everywhere, including large clinical trials
Flèche et al. (2019), Table 16.2. BHPS (Panel; GBR). Based on 4-point change on a 0–12 scale
Health
Mental health
From excellent to poor mental health (self-rated)
\(-\) 3.13 (± 0.30)
Unknown
Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims
Shi et al. (2019), CCHS 2009–10 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Obtained from control variables
Health
Nutrition
From 0 to 8 portions of fruit and vegetables a day
+ 0.16 (± 0.08)
Unknown
Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims
Shi et al. (2019), Table 2, column 2. CCHS 2009–10 (Cross-sectional; CAN)
Health
Physical health
From excellent to poor physical health (self-rated)
\(-\) 2.19 (±  0.17)
Unknown
Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims
Shi et al. (2019), Table 2, Column 1. CCHS 2009–10 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Obtained from control variables
Health
Physical health
From healthy to poor physical health (self-rated)
\(-\) 0.96
Permanent effect, with initial peak
High as found everywhere, including to health shocks
Carbonell and Ada, and Paul Frijters, (2004), Unclear but likely taken from Table 3. See additional comments column. GSOEP 1983–2011 (Panel; DEU). Based on a 3-point change in a 1–5 self-report measure of physical health
Health
Physical health
From healthy to poor physical health (self-rated)
\(-\) 1.080 (±  0.122)
Permanent effect, with initial peak
High as found everywhere, including to health shocks
Frijters et al. (2014), Table 4, column 2. NCDS 1958–2009 (Panel; GBR)
Health
Physical health
Satisfied with health status, at age 60 or older
+ 0.292 (± 0.059) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Medium. Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims, yet findings are consistent with many studies suggesting health is the strongest single predictor of late-life SWB
Zelikova (2013), Table 2, Column 7. WVS 2005–07 (Cross-sectional; CAN, NZL, GBR, USA)
Health
Smoking
From smoking daily to not at all
+ 0.12 (±  0.04)
Unknown
Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims
Shi et al. (2019), Table 2, column 1. CCHS 2009–10 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Obtained from control variables
Social capital
Belonging
Sense of belonging to Canada
+ 0.336 (± 0.137) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross sectional data precludes causal claims
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 5. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN). A sense of belonging to Canada is strongly associated with general social trust
Social capital
Belonging
Sense of belonging to the community
+ 0.781 (± 0.110) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross sectional data precludes causal claims but is consistent with broader literature suggesting community-level belonging is most important
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 5. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN). A sense of belonging to one’s community is strongly associated with neighbourhood trust
Social capital
Belonging
Sense of belonging to the province
+ 0.274 (± 0.114) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross sectional data precludes causal claims
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 5. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN)
Social capital
Discrimination
Experience religious discrimination
\(-\) 0.39
Unknown
Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims
Vang et al. (2019), Table 4, Column 2. GSS27 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Significant positive interaction term suggests higher religiosity mitigates the negative effect of religious discrimination
Social capital
Friendships
Can count on friends
+ 0.414 (± 0.090) on 11-point Cantril ladder
Unknown
Low. Cross sectional data with regional effects; causality unclear
Helliwell and Shun (2011), GWP 2006 (Cross-sectional; WLD). Comes from Y/N response to question: “If you were in trouble, do you have relatives or friends you can count on to help you whenever you need them, or not?”
Social capital
Friendships
From 0 close friends to 3–5 close friends
+ 0.241 (± 0.017) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross sectional data; consistent with broader literature
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 1. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Impact is much smaller for those who are married or living with a partner, suggesting friends and spouses provide some similar happiness benefits
Social capital
Friendships
From 0 close relatives to 3–5 close relatives
+ 0.526 (± 0.149) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross sectional data; consistent with broader literature
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 1. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Paper includes several categories of numbers of close relatives (1 or 2, 3–5, 6–10, 11–20, over 20), an increase from one category to the next is about 0.15
Social capital
Friendships
Seeing close friends more frequently
+ 0.096 (± 0.051) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims, but consistent with
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 4. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Frequency of visits with family and especially with friends add significantly to LS above and beyond the effects of having such networks in place
Social capital
Friendships
Seeing close relatives more frequently
+ 0.096 (± 0.051) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross sectional data; consistent with broader literature
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 1. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Frequency of visits with family add significantly to LS above and beyond the effects of having the network in place
Social capital
Immigration
Being an immigrant parent (female)
\(-\) 0.210 (± 0.106) on 5-point LS
No apparent improvement over time, “years since arrival” variable is statistically insignificant
Medium. Cross sectional data, effect persists with controls for personal characteristics such as ethnicity, income, etc; consistent with broader literature
Burton et al. (2010), Table 5, Column 3. CCHS 2002–10 (Cross-sectional; CAN). No statistically significant effect for female immigrant children once mediating variables (language, ethnicity) are added
Social capital
Immigration
Being an immigrant parent (male)
\(-\) 0.218 (± 0.133) on 5-point LS
No apparent improvement over time, “years since arrival” variable is statistically insignificant
Medium. Cross sectional data, effect persists with controls for personal characteristics such as ethnicity, income, etc; consistent with broader literature
Burton et al. (2010), Table 5, Column 4. CCHS 2002–10 (Cross-sectional; CAN). No statistically significant effect for female immigrant children once mediating variables (language, ethnicity) are added
Social capital
Romantic relationships
From never married to married at 50 or older
+ 0.20 (± \(-\) 0.078)
Permanent effect with high initial peak
Medium: cohort study findings so causality unclear
Flèche et al. (2019), Table 9.1. BHPS (Panel; GBR)
Social capital
Romantic relationships
From partnered to separated
\(-\) 0.40 (± \(-\) 0.14)
High intial effect, then some adaptation
High as found everywhere
Flèche et al. (2019), Table 5.2. BHPS (Panel; GBR). Note that most find new partners and don’t stay separated. Lone men suffer more
Social capital
Romantic relationships
From single to married/partnered
+ 0.28 (± \(-\) 0.10)
Permanent effect with initial peak
High. Ubiquitous finding around the world
Flèche et al. (2019), Table 5.2. BHPS (Panel; GBR)
Social capital
Romantic relationships
From single to married/partnered
+ 0.1
Permanent effect with initial peak
High. Ubiquitous finding around the world
Carbonell and Ada, and Paul Frijters, (2004), Taken from Frijters and Krekel’s table—not exactly sure where this coefficient came from. Maybe Column 1: fixed effect ordered logit 0.08 in Table 3 ?. GSOEP 1983–2011 (Panel; DEU)
Social capital
Romantic relationships
From single to married/partnered
+  0.60 (± 0.022)
Unknown
High. Panel data, fixed instrumental effects
Latif (2010), Table 3, Column 2. NPHS 1994–2007, CCHS 2009–11 (Panel; CAN)
Social capital
Romantic relationships
Never married, age 60 or older
\(-\) 0.122 (± \(-\) 0.078)
Unknown
Medium. Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims, yet consistent with broader literature as found widely
Zelikova (2013), Table 2, Column 7. WVS 2005–07 (Cross-sectional data; CAN, NZL, GBR, USA)
Social capital
Trust
Believe a lost wallet is likely to be returned if found by a stranger
+  0.237 (±  0.098) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross sectional data precludes causal claims but is consistent with GWP findings and broader literature
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 3. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN)
Social capital
Trust
Believe a lost wallet is likely to be returned if found by a stranger
+ 0.074 (± 0.098) on 11-point Cantril ladder
Unknown
Low. Cross sectional data includes regional fixed effects; but effect is statistically insignificant
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 2-c, Column 6. GWP 2006 (Cross-sectional; WLD)
Social capital
Trust
Believe a lost wallet is likely to be returned if found by neighbours
+ 0.172 (± 0.088) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross sectional data; consistent with GWP findings and broader literature
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 3. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Respondents who live in high-density census tracts and are highly mobile are less likely to believe a neighbour would return their wallet
Social capital
Trust
Believe a lost wallet is likely to be returned if found by neighbours
0.117 (± 0.088) on 11-point Cantril ladder
Unknown
Medium. Cross sectional data includes regional fixed effects; generally consistent with broader literature
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 2-a, Column 6. GWP 2006 (Cross-sectional; WLD)
Social capital
Trust
Believe a lost wallet is likely to be returned if found by police
0.138 (± 0.094) on 11-point Cantril ladder
Unknown
Medium. Cross sectional data includes regional fixed effects; generally consistent with broader literature
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 2-b, Column 6. GWP 2006 (Cross-sectional; WLD)
Social capital
Trust
Confidence in police
+ 0.361 (± 0.114) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross sectional data precludes causal claims
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 5. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN)
Social capital
Trust
Social trust (self-reported trust in“most people”)
+ 0.131 on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims; statistically significant positive effect on life satisfaction and domain satisfaction in all domains
van der Horst and Coffé (2012), Table 3, Column 1. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Social trust measured by a binary variable where 0 is“one cannot be too careful in dealing with people”and 1 is “most people can be trusted”
Social capital
Trust
Trust in co-workers
+ 0.638 (± 0.149) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross sectional data precludes causal claims;
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 5. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN)
Social capital
Trust
Trust in neighbours
+ 0.336 (± 0.140) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Cross sectional data precludes causal claims but is consistent with broader literature on community-level trust
Helliwell and Shun (2011), Table 3, Column 5. GSS17 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Respondents who live in high-density census tracts and are highly mobile are less likely to trust their neighbours
Work
Commute
From no commute to 1 h car commute
\(-\) 0.012 (±  0.041)
Unknown
Low. Findings disputed and causality unclear
Dickerson et al. (2014), Table 2, Column 2. BHPS 1996–2008 (Panel; GBR)
Work
Commute
From no commute to 1 h car commute
\(-\) 0.20 (±  0.098)
Unknown
Low. Findings disputed and causality unclear
Stutzer and Frey (2008), Table 1, Column 2. GSOEP 1985–2003 (Panel; DEU)
Work
Commute
Increase in commute (by ???)
\(-\) 0.18 (±  0.1176) on 10-point LS
Unknown
Low. Unclear units on time allocation commuting variable
Hilbrecht et al. (2014), Table 12, Column 2. GSS 24 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Particularly strong effect for women; Significant indirect effects for time spent in physically active leisure and seriousness of traffic congestion
Work
Employment status
From employment to unemployment
\(-\) 0.71 (± 0.059)
Immediate effect higher then reducing, but no adaptation
Immediate effect higher then reducing, but no adaptation
Flèche et al. (2019), Table 4.2. BCS70 (Panel; GBR)
Work
Employment status
From employment to unemployment
\(-\) 0.46 (± 0.078)
Immediate effect higher, then reducing, but no adaptation
High. Large effects found in longitudinal cross-sections, recession-related and employment-shock related (plant closures)
Flèche et al. (2019), Table 4.2. GSEOP (Panel; DEU)
Work
Employment status
From employment to unemployment
− .054 (± 0.022) on 5-point happiness-in-life
Short and long term effects
High. Panel data, fixed instrumental effects
Latif (2010), Table 3, Column 2. NPHS 1994–2007, CCHS 2009–11 (Panel; CAN). Not statistically significant for individuals aged 54 and older
Work
Employment status
From full-time employed to part-time employed not wanting more hours
+ 0.080 (± 0.043)
Largely permanent
Effect very robust in cross section and panels, but causality unclear
De Neve and Ward (2017), Table 6.3, Column 8 “NA+ANZ”. GWP 2006–08 (Cross-sectional and panel; CAN, NZL, AUS, USA). Particularly strong effect for men
Work
Employment status
From full-time employed to part-time employed wanting more hours
\(-\) 0.108 (± 0.016)
Largely permanent
Effect very robust in cross section and panels, but causality unclear
De Neve and Ward (2017), Table 6.3, Column 8 “NA+ANZ”. GWP 2006–08 (Cross-sectional and panel; CAN, NZL, AUS, USA). Particularly strong effect for men
Work
Employment status
From unemployment to out-of-labour force
\(-\) 0.23 (± 0.13)
Unknown
Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims
Shi et al. (2019), Table 4.2. CCHS 2009–10 (Cross-sectional; CAN)
Work
Employment status
From working to retired (at age 55 or older)
+ 0.056 (± 0.047) on 5-point happiness-in-life
Unknown
High. Panel data, fixed instrumental effects
Latif (2011), Table 2, Column 4. NPHS 1994–2007 (Panel; CAN). No significant effect for ages 45–54
Work
Job satisfaction
One unit change on 0–10 scale of non-financial job satisfaction
+ 0.15 (± 0.04)
Unknown
Cross sectional data but findings consistent between ESC and GSS data. Causality unclear
Helliwell and Huang (2010), Table 1, Column 2. GSS17, ESC2 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Income effect instrumented for ESC data, adjusted in GSS data
Work
Type of job
Being in a white collar job versus a blue collar job
Approx. + 0.80
Unknown
Effect very robust in cross-section and panels but causality unclear
De Neve and Ward (2017), Approximated from job categories in Table 6.5 (?). GWP 2006–08 (Cross-sectional and panel; WLD). White collar includes: managers, officials, clerical and office workers; blue collar includes construction, transportation, farming
Work
Type of job
Employment in an occupation that is below an individual’s skills or work experience (immigrants)
\(-\) 0.055 (± 0.096)
Negative effect tends to diminish with increased length of stay in Canada
Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims
Hou et al. (2017), Table 3, Column 4. CCHS 2009–14 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Lower income the main intermediate factor linking over-education to life satisfaction for immigrant
Work
Type of job
Employment in an occupation that is below an individual’s skills or work experience (non-immigrants)
\(-\) 0.280 (± 0.049)
Unknown
Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims
Hou et al. (2017), Table 3, Column 2. CCHS 2009–14 (Cross-sectional; CAN). Lower income just one of the important factors for non-immigrants
Work
Work conditions
Flexible work hours
+ 0.19 (± 0.1176)
Unknown
Cross-sectional data precludes causal claims
Hilbrecht et al. (2014), Table 12, Column 3. GSS 24 (Cross-sectional; CAN)
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Abdel-Khalek, A. M. (2006). Measuring happiness with a single-item scale. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 34(2), 139–150.CrossRef Abdel-Khalek, A. M. (2006). Measuring happiness with a single-item scale. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 34(2), 139–150.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Aknin, L. B., Barrington-Leigh, C. P., Dunn, E. W., Helliwell, J. F., Burns, J., Biswas-Diener, R., Kemeza, I., Nyende, P., Ashton-James, C. E., & Norton, M. I. (2013). Prosocial spending and well-being: Cross-cultural evidence for a psychological universal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 635–652.CrossRef Aknin, L. B., Barrington-Leigh, C. P., Dunn, E. W., Helliwell, J. F., Burns, J., Biswas-Diener, R., Kemeza, I., Nyende, P., Ashton-James, C. E., & Norton, M. I. (2013). Prosocial spending and well-being: Cross-cultural evidence for a psychological universal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 635–652.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Barrington-Leigh, C. P. (2021). Life satisfaction and sustainability: A policy framework. SN Social Sciences. Barrington-Leigh, C. P. (2021). Life satisfaction and sustainability: A policy framework. SN Social Sciences.
Zurück zum Zitat Barrington-Leigh, C. P. (2022). Trends in conceptions of progress and wellbeing. In: J. Helliwell, R. Layard, J. Sachs, J. E. De Neve, L. B. Aknin, & S. Wang (Eds.), World Happiness Report 2022 (Vol. 3, pp. 53–74). Sustainable Development Solutions Network. Barrington-Leigh, C. P. (2022). Trends in conceptions of progress and wellbeing. In: J. Helliwell, R. Layard, J. Sachs, J. E. De Neve, L. B. Aknin, & S. Wang (Eds.), World Happiness Report 2022 (Vol. 3, pp. 53–74). Sustainable Development Solutions Network.
Zurück zum Zitat Barrington-Leigh, C., & Behzadnejad, F. (2017). Evaluating the short-term cost of low-level local air pollution: A life satisfaction approach. Environmental Economics and Policy Studies, 19(2), 269–298.CrossRef Barrington-Leigh, C., & Behzadnejad, F. (2017). Evaluating the short-term cost of low-level local air pollution: A life satisfaction approach. Environmental Economics and Policy Studies, 19(2), 269–298.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Barrington-Leigh, C., & Behzadnejad, F. (2017). The impact of daily weather conditions on life satisfaction: Evidence from cross-sectional and panel data. Journal of Economic Psychology, 59, 145–163.CrossRef Barrington-Leigh, C., & Behzadnejad, F. (2017). The impact of daily weather conditions on life satisfaction: Evidence from cross-sectional and panel data. Journal of Economic Psychology, 59, 145–163.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brzozowski, M., & Visano, B. S. (2020). Havin’ money’s not everything, not havin’ it is: The importance of financial satisfaction for life satisfaction in financially stressed households’. Journal of Happiness Studies, 21(2), 573–591.CrossRef Brzozowski, M., & Visano, B. S. (2020). Havin’ money’s not everything, not havin’ it is: The importance of financial satisfaction for life satisfaction in financially stressed households’. Journal of Happiness Studies, 21(2), 573–591.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Burton, P., & Shelley, P. (2010). The well-being of immigrant children and parents in Canada. Working Paper, Dalhousie University, Department of Economics Burton, P., & Shelley, P. (2010). The well-being of immigrant children and parents in Canada. Working Paper, Dalhousie University, Department of Economics
Zurück zum Zitat Carbonell, F. A., & Frijters, P. (2004). How important is methodology for the estimates of the determinants of happiness? The Economic Journal, 114(497), 641–659.CrossRef Carbonell, F. A., & Frijters, P. (2004). How important is methodology for the estimates of the determinants of happiness? The Economic Journal, 114(497), 641–659.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Clark, A. E., & Jung, S. (2017). Does compulsory education really increase life satisfaction? Technical Report 2017-6. Institute of Business and Economic Research, Inha University. Clark, A. E., & Jung, S. (2017). Does compulsory education really increase life satisfaction? Technical Report 2017-6. Institute of Business and Economic Research, Inha University.
Zurück zum Zitat Clark, A. E., Flèche, S., Layard, R., Powdthavee, N., & Ward, G. (2019). The origins of happiness: The science of well-being over the life course. Princeton University Press.CrossRef Clark, A. E., Flèche, S., Layard, R., Powdthavee, N., & Ward, G. (2019). The origins of happiness: The science of well-being over the life course. Princeton University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat De Neve, J.-E., & Ward, G. (2017). Happiness at work. In J. F. Helliwell, R. Layard, & J. Sachs (Eds.), World happiness report. Sustainable Development Solutions Network. De Neve, J.-E., & Ward, G. (2017). Happiness at work. In J. F. Helliwell, R. Layard, & J. Sachs (Eds.), World happiness report. Sustainable Development Solutions Network.
Zurück zum Zitat Department of Finance. (2021). Toward a quality of life strategy for Canada. Technical Report (p. 4). Government of Canada. Department of Finance. (2021). Toward a quality of life strategy for Canada. Technical Report (p. 4). Government of Canada.
Zurück zum Zitat Dickerson, A., Hole, A. R., & Munford, L. A. (2014). The relationship between well-being and commuting revisited: Does the choice of methodology matter? Regional Science and Urban Economics, 49, 321–329.CrossRef Dickerson, A., Hole, A. R., & Munford, L. A. (2014). The relationship between well-being and commuting revisited: Does the choice of methodology matter? Regional Science and Urban Economics, 49, 321–329.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Flèche, S., Clark, A. E., Layard, R., Powdthavee, N., & Ward, G. (2019). The origins of happiness: The science of well-being over the life course. Princeton University Press. Flèche, S., Clark, A. E., Layard, R., Powdthavee, N., & Ward, G. (2019). The origins of happiness: The science of well-being over the life course. Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Frijters, P., Clark, A. E., Krekel, C., & Layard, R. (2020). A happy choice: Wellbeing as the goal of government. Behavioural Public Policy, 4(2), 126–165.CrossRef Frijters, P., Clark, A. E., Krekel, C., & Layard, R. (2020). A happy choice: Wellbeing as the goal of government. Behavioural Public Policy, 4(2), 126–165.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Frijters, P., Haisken-DeNew, J. P., & Shields, M. A. (2004). Money does matter! Evidence from increasing real income and life satisfaction in east Germany following reunification. The American Economic Review, 94(3), 730–740.CrossRef Frijters, P., Haisken-DeNew, J. P., & Shields, M. A. (2004). Money does matter! Evidence from increasing real income and life satisfaction in east Germany following reunification. The American Economic Review, 94(3), 730–740.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Frijters, P., Johnston, D. W., & Shields, M. A. (2014). Does childhood predict adult life satisfaction? Evidence from British cohort surveys. The Economic Journal, 124(580), F688–F719.CrossRef Frijters, P., Johnston, D. W., & Shields, M. A. (2014). Does childhood predict adult life satisfaction? Evidence from British cohort surveys. The Economic Journal, 124(580), F688–F719.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Frijters, P., & Krekel, C. (2021). A handbook for wellbeing policy-making: History, theory, measurement, implementation, and examples. Oxford University Press.CrossRef Frijters, P., & Krekel, C. (2021). A handbook for wellbeing policy-making: History, theory, measurement, implementation, and examples. Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hanslmaier, M. (2013). Crime, fear and subjective well-being: How victimization and street crime affect fear and life satisfaction. European Journal of Criminology, 10(5), 515–533.CrossRef Hanslmaier, M. (2013). Crime, fear and subjective well-being: How victimization and street crime affect fear and life satisfaction. European Journal of Criminology, 10(5), 515–533.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Happiness Research Institute. (2020). Wellbeing adjusted life years: A universal metric to quantify the happiness return on investment. Happiness Research Institute. (2020). Wellbeing adjusted life years: A universal metric to quantify the happiness return on investment.
Zurück zum Zitat Helliwell, J. F., & Wang, S. (2011). Trust and wellbeing. International Journal of Wellbeing. Helliwell, J. F., & Wang, S. (2011). Trust and wellbeing. International Journal of Wellbeing.
Zurück zum Zitat Helliwell, J., Shun, W., Haifang, H., & Max N. (2022). Happiness, benevolence, and trust during covid-19 and beyond. In J. F. Helliwell, R. Layard, J. D. Sachs, J. E. De Neve, L. B. Aknin, & S. Wang (Eds.), World happiness report 2022 (Vol. 2, pp. 13–52). Sustainable Development Solutions Network. Helliwell, J., Shun, W., Haifang, H., & Max N. (2022). Happiness, benevolence, and trust during covid-19 and beyond. In J. F. Helliwell, R. Layard, J. D. Sachs, J. E. De Neve, L. B. Aknin, & S. Wang (Eds.), World happiness report 2022 (Vol. 2, pp. 13–52). Sustainable Development Solutions Network.
Zurück zum Zitat Helliwell, J. F., & Huang, H. (2010). How’s the job? Well-being and social capital in the workplace. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 63(2), 205–227.CrossRef Helliwell, J. F., & Huang, H. (2010). How’s the job? Well-being and social capital in the workplace. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 63(2), 205–227.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hilbrecht, M., Smale, B., & Mock, S. E. (2014). Highway to health? Commute time and well-being among Canadian adults. World Leisure Journal, 56(2), 151–163.CrossRef Hilbrecht, M., Smale, B., & Mock, S. E. (2014). Highway to health? Commute time and well-being among Canadian adults. World Leisure Journal, 56(2), 151–163.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hou, F., & Kristyn F. (2017). Over-education and life satisfaction among immigrant and non-immigrant workers in Canada. Technical Report 2017393e. Statistics Canada, Analytical Studies Branch. Hou, F., & Kristyn F. (2017). Over-education and life satisfaction among immigrant and non-immigrant workers in Canada. Technical Report 2017393e. Statistics Canada, Analytical Studies Branch.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnston, D. W., Shields, M. A., & Suziedelyte, A. (2018). Victimisation, well-being and compensation: Using panel data to estimate the costs of violent crime. The Economic Journal, 128(611), 1545–1569.CrossRef Johnston, D. W., Shields, M. A., & Suziedelyte, A. (2018). Victimisation, well-being and compensation: Using panel data to estimate the costs of violent crime. The Economic Journal, 128(611), 1545–1569.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Krekel, C., Kolbe, J., & Wüstemann, H. (2016). The greener, the happier? The effect of urban land use on residential well-being. Ecological Economics, 121, 117–127.CrossRef Krekel, C., Kolbe, J., & Wüstemann, H. (2016). The greener, the happier? The effect of urban land use on residential well-being. Ecological Economics, 121, 117–127.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Krekel, C., & Zerrahn, A. (2017). Does the presence of wind turbines have negative externalities for people in their surroundings? Evidence from well-being data. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 82, 221–238.CrossRef Krekel, C., & Zerrahn, A. (2017). Does the presence of wind turbines have negative externalities for people in their surroundings? Evidence from well-being data. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 82, 221–238.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Latif, E. (2010). Crisis, unemployment and psychological wellbeing in Canada. Journal of Policy Modeling, 32(4), 520–530.CrossRef Latif, E. (2010). Crisis, unemployment and psychological wellbeing in Canada. Journal of Policy Modeling, 32(4), 520–530.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Latif, E. (2011). The impact of retirement on psychological well-being in Canada. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 40(4), 373–380.CrossRef Latif, E. (2011). The impact of retirement on psychological well-being in Canada. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 40(4), 373–380.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Latif, E. (2016). Happiness and comparison income: Evidence from Canada. Social Indicators Research, 128(1), 161–177.CrossRef Latif, E. (2016). Happiness and comparison income: Evidence from Canada. Social Indicators Research, 128(1), 161–177.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Levinson, A. (2012). Valuing public goods using happiness data: The case of air quality. Journal of Public Economics, 96(9–10), 869–880.CrossRef Levinson, A. (2012). Valuing public goods using happiness data: The case of air quality. Journal of Public Economics, 96(9–10), 869–880.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Luechinger, S. (2009). Valuing air quality using the life satisfaction approach. The Economic Journal, 119(536), 482–515.CrossRef Luechinger, S. (2009). Valuing air quality using the life satisfaction approach. The Economic Journal, 119(536), 482–515.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mujcic, R., & Oswald, A. J. (2016). Evolution of well-being and happiness after increases in consumption of fruit and vegetables. American Journal of Public Health, 106(8), 1504–1510.CrossRef Mujcic, R., & Oswald, A. J. (2016). Evolution of well-being and happiness after increases in consumption of fruit and vegetables. American Journal of Public Health, 106(8), 1504–1510.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat OECD. (2013). OECD guidelines on measuring subjective well-being. OECD Publishing. OECD. (2013). OECD guidelines on measuring subjective well-being. OECD Publishing.
Zurück zum Zitat Shi, Y., Joyce, C., Wall, R., Orpana, H., & Bancej, C. (2019). A life satisfaction approach to valuing the impact of health behaviours on subjective well-being. BMC Public Health, 19(1), 1547.CrossRef Shi, Y., Joyce, C., Wall, R., Orpana, H., & Bancej, C. (2019). A life satisfaction approach to valuing the impact of health behaviours on subjective well-being. BMC Public Health, 19(1), 1547.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stone, A. A., Christopher, M., et al. (2014). Subjective well-being: Measuring happiness, suffering, and other dimensions of experience. National Academies Press. Stone, A. A., Christopher, M., et al. (2014). Subjective well-being: Measuring happiness, suffering, and other dimensions of experience. National Academies Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Stutzer, A., & Frey, B. S. (2008). Stress that doesn’t pay: The commuting paradox*. The Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 110(2), 339–366.CrossRef Stutzer, A., & Frey, B. S. (2008). Stress that doesn’t pay: The commuting paradox*. The Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 110(2), 339–366.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Treasury, U. K. (2021). Wellbeing guidance for appraisal: Supplementary green book guidance. Technical Report (p. 7). UK Government. Treasury, U. K. (2021). Wellbeing guidance for appraisal: Supplementary green book guidance. Technical Report (p. 7). UK Government.
Zurück zum Zitat van der Horst, M., & Coffé, H. (2012). How friendship network characteristics influence subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 107(3), 509–529.CrossRef van der Horst, M., & Coffé, H. (2012). How friendship network characteristics influence subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 107(3), 509–529.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Vang, Z. M., Hou, F., & Elder, K. (2019). Perceived religious discrimination, religiosity, and life satisfaction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 20(6), 1913–1932.CrossRef Vang, Z. M., Hou, F., & Elder, K. (2019). Perceived religious discrimination, religiosity, and life satisfaction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 20(6), 1913–1932.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Veenhoven, R. World database of happiness. Veenhoven, R. World database of happiness.
Zurück zum Zitat What Works Centre for Wellbeing. (2018). Wellbeing in policy analysis. Technical Report, 3. What Works Centre for Wellbeing. (2018). Wellbeing in policy analysis. Technical Report, 3.
Zurück zum Zitat White, M. P., Alcock, I., Wheeler, B. W., & Depledge, M. H. (2013). Would you be happier living in a greener urban area? A fixed-effects analysis of panel data. Psychological Science, 24(6), 920–928.CrossRef White, M. P., Alcock, I., Wheeler, B. W., & Depledge, M. H. (2013). Would you be happier living in a greener urban area? A fixed-effects analysis of panel data. Psychological Science, 24(6), 920–928.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zelikova, J. (2013). Successful aging: A cross-national study of subjective well-being later in life. HSE Working Paper WP BRP 21/SOC/2013. National Research University Higher School of Economics. Zelikova, J. (2013). Successful aging: A cross-national study of subjective well-being later in life. HSE Working Paper WP BRP 21/SOC/2013. National Research University Higher School of Economics.
Metadaten
Titel
A Public, Open, and Independently-Curated Database of Happiness Coefficients
verfasst von
C. P. Barrington-Leigh
Katja Lemermeyer
Publikationsdatum
03.04.2023
Verlag
Springer Netherlands
Erschienen in
Journal of Happiness Studies / Ausgabe 4/2023
Print ISSN: 1389-4978
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-7780
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-023-00652-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2023

Journal of Happiness Studies 4/2023 Zur Ausgabe