Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Political Behavior 2/2019

10.04.2018 | Original Paper

What I Like About You: Legislator Personality and Legislator Approval

verfasst von: Jonathan D. Klingler, Gary E. Hollibaugh Jr., Adam J. Ramey

Erschienen in: Political Behavior | Ausgabe 2/2019

Einloggen

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Recent work in the study of legislative politics has uncovered associations between the Big Five personality traits and myriad phenomena in the United States Congress. This literature raises new questions about political representation in terms of the Big Five, specifically, whether voters are more likely to support legislators with similar personality traits to their own, who would presumably have similar process preferences, or legislators with valence personality traits, regardless of congruence, which are associated with better leadership. We first revisit the measurement validity of voter assessments of legislator personality in the 2014 and 2016 Cooperative Congressional Election Studies to demonstrate that such survey items are meaningful. Subsequently, we use these data to construct measures of personality congruence and valence and apply them to predict voters’ job approval of legislators. Our results support the claim that voters evaluate legislators’ job performance on the basis of perceived valence traits rather than legislators’ congruence to voters’ own personality dispositions.

Sie haben noch keine Lizenz? Dann Informieren Sie sich jetzt über unsere Produkte:

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft+Technik" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 102.000 Bücher
  • über 537 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Automobil + Motoren
  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Elektrotechnik + Elektronik
  • Energie + Nachhaltigkeit
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Maschinenbau + Werkstoffe
  • Versicherung + Risiko

Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Springer Professional "Wirtschaft"

Online-Abonnement

Mit Springer Professional "Wirtschaft" erhalten Sie Zugriff auf:

  • über 67.000 Bücher
  • über 340 Zeitschriften

aus folgenden Fachgebieten:

  • Bauwesen + Immobilien
  • Business IT + Informatik
  • Finance + Banking
  • Management + Führung
  • Marketing + Vertrieb
  • Versicherung + Risiko




Jetzt Wissensvorsprung sichern!

Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
Primary elections (Brady et al. 2007) mitigate these effects in the general election, largely because of ideological convergence concerns among the primary electorate.
 
2
Enelow and Hinich (1982) further discretized valence attributes, breaking them down into valence attributes—immutable attributes on which all voters hold the same evaluation—and ascriptive attributes—immutable attributes on which the evaluations of voters differ—with one of the primary distinctions between the types of attributes being that candidates can choose position attributes, while valence and ascriptive attributes cannot be selected.
 
3
The underlying basic preferences captured by personality measures such as the Big Five should influence the emphases legislators put on their careers—at home or in Washington—and perhaps the relative costs of those leadership styles. However, other influences, such as those mentioned by Fenno (1978), are mitigating factors as politicians choose how to allocate their resources during their career. While home style may be highly influenced by personality, we would not go so far as to venture that home style is personality.
 
4
Moreover, it is not just voters who care about the competence, integrity, effort, and effectiveness of officeholders. When choosing those appointees ultimately responsible for policy implementation, executives have long weighed competence—and other nonpolicy factors—as important considerations necessary to effective policymaking (Aberbach and Rockman 2000; Gailmard and Patty 2007; Hollibaugh et al. 2014; Hollibaugh 2015a, b; Huber and McCarty 2004; Lewis 2008, 2009; Moe 1985; Nathan 1983).
 
5
Also see McCurley and Mondak (1995).
 
6
There may be links between voter personality and legislator approval beyond personality valence or congruence. For example, voters with certain personality types may differ in the policy preferences they desire from legislators, following the argument of Caprara and Zimbardo (2004). Additionally, certain Big Five personality types may exhibit different preferences over legislator valence characteristics such as character or leadership ability. These questions are worthy of further examination, but are beyond the representation focus of this project.
 
7
While the TIPI is shorter than standard instruments, it is well-suited to tasks like the CCES, where time is limited, and results from the TIPI tend to be highly correlated with the results one would get from longer and larger batteries of questions (Ehrhart et al. 2009; Gosling et al. 2003). The question wording is in Online Appendix I.
 
8
One concern that arises here is that we are relying on respondents to sincerely place the personalities of their elected officials. Specifically, one might be concerned that there might be a halo effect, as suggested by Caprara et al. (2007), whereby respondents assign “good” personality types to legislators who they already like. This can induce a form of simultaneity bias. We show in Online Appendix L that this concern is not substantial. If present, such a bias is shown in the Online Appendix to be always downward in nature.
 
9
Unfortunately, because ELUCIDATION scores do not exist past 2014, we do not include the 2016 CCES in our validation procedure. Moreover, due to the way ELUCIDATION scores are calculated, we do not have any scores for those whose first—or only—session of Congress was the 113th.
 
10
Both ideology and personality questions utilize seven-point scales, thus ensuring some degree of scale comparability for our ideological and personality measures.
 
11
There may be some concerns that respondents may differ in their abilities to rate legislator personality and may exhibit different relationships between legislator personality ratings and approval on the basis of respondent political information. In Online Appendices G and H, we separately examine high and low information voters and find little difference between the two groups with respect to the relationship between legislator personality and approval.
 
12
Our measures are subjective and thus based entirely on the perceptions of respondents about their own personality and the personalities of their legislators. While it would be interesting to examine the relationship between personality congruence and valence between voters and elected officials as measured by experts or longer, more reliable inventories, our objective is to test for possible associations between job approval and perceived personality congruence/valence in the minds of voters, and for this task our measures are very well suited.
 
13
Despite the previous validity checks, there might be some residual concern that endogeneity plagues our analyses. However, the structure of the 2014 CCES somewhat mitigates this concern. For the Representatives and half of the Senators, the TIPI questions were asked prior to the approval questions, and both were asked during the pre-election wave. However, for the remaining Senators (denoted here as “Senators 2”), the TIPI questions were asked during the post-election wave, and the approval questions were asked during the pre-election wave. While this setup (unfortunately) forces us to use post-election questions as explanatory variables where a pre-election question is the dependent variable, separate regressions where the two Senators are estimated separately (for the 2014 panel) provide substantively identical results, though the results for are somewhat weaker for the Senator 2 analysis (see Online Appendix D). These results not only suggest that there are no serious problems with using a post-election variable as an independent variable in the present case, but also that the aformentioned endogeneity issue is of little practical concern.
 
14
It is possible that causality may flow in the opposite direction and voters may state that politicians of whom they already approve display “positive” personality types. In this case, it would only uncover valence properties of incumbent personality through another pathway, as these voters are implicitly stating their preference for elected officials with these personality trait profiles. This is more likely when voters have low amounts of information about candidates, but in this project we are working with evaluations of incumbent legislators, making this unlikely. That said, we have also conducted analyses using voters’ evaulations of President Obama (using the 2016 sample, since voters were not asked to rate the personality of President Obama in the 2014 sample). While the relationships between voters’ evaluations of President Obama’s personality traits and their approval evaluations are somewhat noiser than the legislator models (perhaps due to the smaller sample or less malleable opinions about a President in his eighth year), the overall findings for the Obama-specific models are substantively similar to those found for the Congressional models. These results are in Online Appendix C.
 
15
Also see Hollibaugh et al. (2012).
 
16
However, since party likely plays a strong role independent of ideology, we replicate our main results focusing only on those legislator-respondent dyads where the legislators and respondents are of the same party, as well as a separate subset where the legislators and respondents are not of the same party. These results, which are in Online Appendices E (copartisans) and F (outpartisans), are substantively similar to those presented in the main body of the paper.
 
17
While theoretically possible, we do not perform Ramey-scaling on the personality variables. This is for several reasons. First, both the Aldrich and McKelvey (1977) method, as well as Ramey’s (2016) modification thereof, require that the analyst choose an individual stimulus a priori to anchor the recovered space. In the context of ideology, this is relatively straightforward, since many reliable measures of ideology exist (e.g., Clinton et al. 2004; Poole and Rosenthal 1997) and only one dimension (the underlying liberal-conservative dimension) needs to be estimated. In the context of personality scores, reliable outside measures of all personality traits are scarce, and even the ELUCIDATION scores might be somewhat problematic for this purpose, since while they correlate with respondents’ perceptions of personality, the relationship are quite noisy; further recall that there is no relationship between the ELUCIDATION score for Conscientiousness and respondents’ perceptions of how their legislators rate on this trait, which would make it particularly hard to anchor it to any degree of confidence. Moreover, while the Aldrich and McKelvey (1977) method and Ramey’s (2016) modification thereof account for within-respondent correlations across stimuli, they do so in a circumstance where only one latent dimension is being recovered. There is undoubtedly within-subject correlation across the five latent personality dimensions, and properly accounting for that within the broader Aldrich and McKelvey (1977) framework would require the development of an entirely new method, which is beyond the scope of this article. Finally, there are substantive concerns as well. The extant literature on the stability of personality traits over time suggests that the expression of persistent personality trait dispositions is conditioned by stochastic-contextual processes as well the social environments and roles to which individuals are exposed (Fraley and Roberts 2005). For example, an introvert may be quite talkative in the context of a family dinner while exhibiting a reserved demeanor among strangers. The assignment of context by nature is inherently noisy and not necessarily random. As a result, several researchers have referred to personality measures as exhibiting ‘patterns of stability’ or ‘patterns of continuity’ and have accepted a significant degree of variation in these measures (McCrae and Costa 1994; Fraley and Roberts 2005). This inherent variation in any measure of personality makes it particularly difficult to defend the choice of any extreme anchor, and by extension, the use of the Aldrich and McKelvey (1977) method for personality traits.
 
18
In particular, for this latter question, we recorded whether respondents responded correctly, incorrectly, or indicated they did not know to the following statement, “Please indicate whether you’ve heard of this person and if so which party he or she is affiliated with,” for their Governor, both Senators, and Representative. We then recorded each question-response combination (e.g., Governor-Correct, Representative-Incorrect, Senator 1-Don’t Know, etc.) and performed a correspondence analysis on the resulting matrices for each year. We then extracted the first dimension, normalized it to have mean zero and standard deviation one, and used it as our Political Information variable for each year. Results are substantively similar if we instead use a variable that equals one if the respondent indicated he or she “follow[s] what’s going on in government” most of the time and zero otherwise, though our preferred measure allows for more variation. Correspondence analysis was used because the data are inherently categorical due to the presence of both the “Incorrect” and “Don’t Know” possibilities, thus calling into question the underlying ordinality of the responses. Principal components analysis is arguably inappropriate due to the (relative) lack of continuousness. Results are substantively similar if factor analysis is used instead.
 
19
To account for possible differential effects regarding how well-informed respondents are, we estimate models where the set of respondents are those at or above the median on Political Information, and others where the respondents are those below the median. For similar reasons, we also perform analyses limiting our respondent pool to those sharing the party affiliation of the stimuli, and others limiting our pool to those who did not. Results, which are substantively similar, are in Online Appendices E (copartisans) and F (outpartisans).
 
20
We omit the respondent and office-year random effects from office-specific models and Obama models due to the lack of variation in these cases. In these cases, the office-year random effects simply reduce to year-specific random effects, and the lack of variation—each intercept can only take one of two values in these cases—leads to convergence issues. Thus, in order to avoid these problems, we simply omit these random effects.
 
21
Note that the Representative-specific model implicitly accounts for cycle effects, since all Representatives are up for reelection every two years.
 
22
Both of these tests are described in greater detail in Online Appendices J (Clarke Test) and K (Vuong Test).
 
23
In Monte Carlo experiments (Clarke 2007), the Vuong (1989) test was shown to be relatively conservative and to choose the “correct” model less often than Clarke’s (2007) distribution-free test. However, the Clarke (2007) test chooses the “wrong” model more often. Indeed, the Vuong (1989) test’s relative conservatism prevents it from declaring either model to be the “correct” model in a large proportion of cases; when it does choose a model, however, it tends to be correct. The Clarke (2007) test is more powerful and more likely to declare one model fits better than the other, but it also tends to choose the wrong model more often. However, the same Monte Carlo results suggest neither test chooses the wrong model often in absolute terms.
 
24
We focus on the congruence variables here because they are more likely to be affected by measurement error, and the encompassing models will allow us to determine whether any useful signal is buried under the noise—which would likely be the case if congruence were at all predictive of legislator approval in a substantive sense—or whether congruence is of minimal to no consequence.
 
25
The encompassing models themselves are in Online Appendix B.
 
26
However, in some sense the substantive interpretation of the valence-based variables are of secondary importance, relative to the finding that valence seems to outperform congruence on nearly every metric.
 
27
For Fig. 4, the estimated coefficients from the “Ideological Divergence, Personality-Respondent Ideology Interaction, and Political Information” models (Models A-4, A-9, and A-14) are used (see Online Appendix A), as these models are the valence-based models with the lowest BIC among those that include Ideological Divergence as a control variable.
 
28
The results presented are for the “average” respondent, in that the random effects terms are set to zero.
 
29
However, the capacity for altruism can also work in favor of politically divisive tactics. For example, Ramey et al. (2017) find that, in many cases, Agreeableness manifests as a willingness to put one’s own goals and preferences aside for the good of one’s party.
 
30
Once again, we also perform “encompassing” model tests (see Online Appendix B), wherein we add the valence [congruence] variables to the congruence [valence] models and perform likelihood ratio tests to examine them for increased model fit. As before, adding the “valence” measures to “congruence” models increases model fit in every case. However, unlike before, we also find evidence that adding “congruence” measures to “valence” models also increases model fit. Nonetheless, the nonnested model tests suggest that, while both sets of variables have influence in the present analyses, the “valence” measures provide better fit.
 
31
In Fig. 5, all covariates—except for the respondent ideology (and its absolute value) and the personality trait allowed to vary—are set to zero. Respondent Ideology was set to one standard deviation below its mean for “liberal” respondents, at its mean for “moderate” respondents, and to one standard deviation above its mean for “conservative” respondents. This forces the model to consider the situation where both Senators are identical on every dimension, with the exception of the personality trait under analysis. As before, the results presented are for the “average” respondent, in that the random effects terms are set to zero. Finally, for all plots, the estimated coefficients from the “Ideological Divergence, Personality-Respondent Ideology Interaction, and Political Information” model (Model A-34) are used (see Online Appendix A), as this model is the relative valence-based model with the lowest BIC among those that include Ideological Divergence as a control variable.
 
32
Separately valence models were estimated for high and low information voters in Online Appendices G and H. Among ideologically moderate voters in the models pooling all officeholders, high information voters exhibited strong valence preferences for Openness, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness in legislators, while low information voters exhibited somewhat weaker preferences for these three traits, but also a valence preference for Emotional Stability.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Aberbach, J. D., & Rockman, B. A. (2000). In the web of politics: Three decades of the U.S. Federal Executive. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press. Aberbach, J. D., & Rockman, B. A. (2000). In the web of politics: Three decades of the U.S. Federal Executive. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Aldrich, J. H., & McKelvey, R. D. (1977). A method of scaling with applications to the 1968 and 1972 presidential elections. American Political Science Review, 71(1), 111–130.CrossRef Aldrich, J. H., & McKelvey, R. D. (1977). A method of scaling with applications to the 1968 and 1972 presidential elections. American Political Science Review, 71(1), 111–130.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Almlund, M., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J., & Kautz, T. (2011). Personality psychology and economics. In E. A. Hanushek, S. Machin, & L. Woessmann (Eds.), Handbook of the economics of education (pp. 1–181). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Almlund, M., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J., & Kautz, T. (2011). Personality psychology and economics. In E. A. Hanushek, S. Machin, & L. Woessmann (Eds.), Handbook of the economics of education (pp. 1–181). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Zurück zum Zitat Ansolabehere, S., Snyder, J. M., Jr., & Stewart, C., III. (2001). Candidate positioning in U.S. house elections. American Journal of Political Science, 45(1), 136–159.CrossRef Ansolabehere, S., Snyder, J. M., Jr., & Stewart, C., III. (2001). Candidate positioning in U.S. house elections. American Journal of Political Science, 45(1), 136–159.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bafumi, J., & Herron, M. C. (2010). Leapfrog representation and extremism: A study of American voters and their members in congress. American Political Science Review, 104(3), 519–542.CrossRef Bafumi, J., & Herron, M. C. (2010). Leapfrog representation and extremism: A study of American voters and their members in congress. American Political Science Review, 104(3), 519–542.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Borghans, L., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. J., & Ter Weel, B. (2008). The economics and psychology of personality traits. Journal of Human Resources, 43(4), 972–1059.CrossRef Borghans, L., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. J., & Ter Weel, B. (2008). The economics and psychology of personality traits. Journal of Human Resources, 43(4), 972–1059.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1993). Convergence of stranger ratings of personality and intelligence with self-ratings, partner ratings, and measured intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(3), 546–553.CrossRef Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1993). Convergence of stranger ratings of personality and intelligence with self-ratings, partner ratings, and measured intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(3), 546–553.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brady, D. W., Han, H., & Pope, J. C. (2007). Primary elections and candidate ideology: Out of step with the primary electorate? Legislative Studies Quarterly, 32(1), 79–105.CrossRef Brady, D. W., Han, H., & Pope, J. C. (2007). Primary elections and candidate ideology: Out of step with the primary electorate? Legislative Studies Quarterly, 32(1), 79–105.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Canes-Wrone, B., Brady, D. W., & Cogan, J. F. (2002). Out of step, out of office: Electoral accountability and house members’ voting. American Political Science Review, 96(1), 127–140.CrossRef Canes-Wrone, B., Brady, D. W., & Cogan, J. F. (2002). Out of step, out of office: Electoral accountability and house members’ voting. American Political Science Review, 96(1), 127–140.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1999). Personality profiles and political parties. Political Psychology, 20(1), 175–197.CrossRef Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1999). Personality profiles and political parties. Political Psychology, 20(1), 175–197.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Caprara, G. V., Vecchione, M., Barbaranelli, C., & Chris Fraley, R. (2007). When likeness goes with liking: The case of political preference. Political Psychology, 28(5), 609–632.CrossRef Caprara, G. V., Vecchione, M., Barbaranelli, C., & Chris Fraley, R. (2007). When likeness goes with liking: The case of political preference. Political Psychology, 28(5), 609–632.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Caprara, G. V., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2004). Personalizing politics: A congruency model of political preference. American Psychologist, 59(7), 581–594.CrossRef Caprara, G. V., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2004). Personalizing politics: A congruency model of political preference. American Psychologist, 59(7), 581–594.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Caprara, G. V., Schwartz, S., Capanna, C., Vecchione, M., & Barbaranelli, C. (2006). Personality and politics: Values, traits, and political choice. Political Psychology, 27(1), 1–28.CrossRef Caprara, G. V., Schwartz, S., Capanna, C., Vecchione, M., & Barbaranelli, C. (2006). Personality and politics: Values, traits, and political choice. Political Psychology, 27(1), 1–28.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Clarke, K. A. (2001). Testing nonnested models of international relations: Reevaluating realism. American Journal of Political Science, 45(3), 724–744.CrossRef Clarke, K. A. (2001). Testing nonnested models of international relations: Reevaluating realism. American Journal of Political Science, 45(3), 724–744.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Clarke, K. A. (2007). A simple distribution-free test for nonnested model selection. Political Analysis, 15(3), 347–363.CrossRef Clarke, K. A. (2007). A simple distribution-free test for nonnested model selection. Political Analysis, 15(3), 347–363.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Clinton, J., Jackman, S., & Rivers, D. (2004). The statistical analysis of roll call data. American Political Science Review, 98(2), 355–370.CrossRef Clinton, J., Jackman, S., & Rivers, D. (2004). The statistical analysis of roll call data. American Political Science Review, 98(2), 355–370.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dietrich, B. J., Lasley, S., Mondak, J. J., Remmel, M. L., & Turner, J. (2012). Personality and legislative politics: The Big Five trait dimensions among U.S. state legislators. Political Psychology, 33(2), 195–210.CrossRef Dietrich, B. J., Lasley, S., Mondak, J. J., Remmel, M. L., & Turner, J. (2012). Personality and legislative politics: The Big Five trait dimensions among U.S. state legislators. Political Psychology, 33(2), 195–210.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of political action in a democracy. The Journal of Political Economy, 65(2), 135–150.CrossRef Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of political action in a democracy. The Journal of Political Economy, 65(2), 135–150.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Egan, P. J. (2014). “Do something” politics and double-peaked policy preferences. Journal of Politics, 76(2), 333–349.CrossRef Egan, P. J. (2014). “Do something” politics and double-peaked policy preferences. Journal of Politics, 76(2), 333–349.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ehrhart, M. G., Ehrhart, K. H., Roesch, S. C., Chung-Herrera, B. G., Nadler, K., & Bradshaw, K. (2009). Testing the latent factor structure and construct validity of the Ten-Item Personality Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 900–905.CrossRef Ehrhart, M. G., Ehrhart, K. H., Roesch, S. C., Chung-Herrera, B. G., Nadler, K., & Bradshaw, K. (2009). Testing the latent factor structure and construct validity of the Ten-Item Personality Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 900–905.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Enelow, J. M., & Hinich, M. J. (1982). Nonspatial candidate characteristics and electoral competition. Journal of Politics, 44(1), 115–130.CrossRef Enelow, J. M., & Hinich, M. J. (1982). Nonspatial candidate characteristics and electoral competition. Journal of Politics, 44(1), 115–130.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fenno, R. F., Jr. (1978). Home style: House members in their districts. Boston: Little Brown. Fenno, R. F., Jr. (1978). Home style: House members in their districts. Boston: Little Brown.
Zurück zum Zitat Fraley, R. C., & Roberts, B. W. (2005). Patterns of continuity: A dynamic model for conceptualizing the stability of individual differences in psychological constructs across the life course. Psychological Review, 112(1), 60–74.CrossRef Fraley, R. C., & Roberts, B. W. (2005). Patterns of continuity: A dynamic model for conceptualizing the stability of individual differences in psychological constructs across the life course. Psychological Review, 112(1), 60–74.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1988). Friends and strangers: Acquaintanceship, agreement, and the accuracy of personality judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(1), 149–158.CrossRef Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1988). Friends and strangers: Acquaintanceship, agreement, and the accuracy of personality judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(1), 149–158.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Funder, D. C., Kolar, D. C., & Blackman, M. C. (1995). Agreement among judges of personality: Interpersonal relations, similarity, and acquaintanceship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(4), 656–672.CrossRef Funder, D. C., Kolar, D. C., & Blackman, M. C. (1995). Agreement among judges of personality: Interpersonal relations, similarity, and acquaintanceship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(4), 656–672.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Funk, C. L. (1996). The impact of scandal on candidate evaluations: An experimental test of the role of candidate traits. Political Behavior, 18(1), 1–24.CrossRef Funk, C. L. (1996). The impact of scandal on candidate evaluations: An experimental test of the role of candidate traits. Political Behavior, 18(1), 1–24.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Funk, C. L. (1997). Implications of political expertise in candidate trait evaluations. Political Research Quarterly, 50(3), 675–697.CrossRef Funk, C. L. (1997). Implications of political expertise in candidate trait evaluations. Political Research Quarterly, 50(3), 675–697.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Funk, C. L. (1999). Bringing the candidate into models of candidate evaluation. Journal of Politics, 61(3), 700–720.CrossRef Funk, C. L. (1999). Bringing the candidate into models of candidate evaluation. Journal of Politics, 61(3), 700–720.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gailmard, S., & Patty, J. W. (2007). Slackers and zealots: Civil service, policy discretion, and bureaucratic expertise. American Journal of Political Science, 51(4), 873–889.CrossRef Gailmard, S., & Patty, J. W. (2007). Slackers and zealots: Civil service, policy discretion, and bureaucratic expertise. American Journal of Political Science, 51(4), 873–889.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B, Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504–528.CrossRef Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B, Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504–528.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Greenacre, M. (2007). Correspondence analysis in practice. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.CrossRef Greenacre, M. (2007). Correspondence analysis in practice. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Groseclose, T. (2001). A model of candidate location when one candidate has a valence advantage. American Journal of Political Science, 45(4), 862–886.CrossRef Groseclose, T. (2001). A model of candidate location when one candidate has a valence advantage. American Journal of Political Science, 45(4), 862–886.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hall, M. E. K. (2015). Judging with personality: The justices’ personality traits and decision making on the U.S. Supreme Court. Unpublished manuscript. Hall, M. E. K. (2015). Judging with personality: The justices’ personality traits and decision making on the U.S. Supreme Court. Unpublished manuscript.
Zurück zum Zitat Hetherington, M. J. (1998). The political relevance of political trust. American Political Science Review, 92(4), 791–808.CrossRef Hetherington, M. J. (1998). The political relevance of political trust. American Political Science Review, 92(4), 791–808.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hetherington, M. J. (2005). Why trust matters: Declining political trust and the demise of American liberalism. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Hetherington, M. J. (2005). Why trust matters: Declining political trust and the demise of American liberalism. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Hetherington, M. J., & Husser, J. A. (2012). How trust matters: The changing political relevance of political trust. American Journal of Political Science, 56(2), 312–325.CrossRef Hetherington, M. J., & Husser, J. A. (2012). How trust matters: The changing political relevance of political trust. American Journal of Political Science, 56(2), 312–325.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hetherington, M. J., & Rudolph, T. J. (2015). Why Washington won’t work: Polarization, political trust, and the governing crisis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef Hetherington, M. J., & Rudolph, T. J. (2015). Why Washington won’t work: Polarization, political trust, and the governing crisis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Holian, D. B., & Prysby, C. (2014). Candidate character traits in the 2012 presidential election. Presidential Studies Quarterly, 44(3), 484–505.CrossRef Holian, D. B., & Prysby, C. (2014). Candidate character traits in the 2012 presidential election. Presidential Studies Quarterly, 44(3), 484–505.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hollibaugh, G. E., Jr. (2015a). Naïve cronyism and neutral competence: Patronage, performance, and policy agreement in executive appointments. Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 25(2), 341–372.CrossRef Hollibaugh, G. E., Jr. (2015a). Naïve cronyism and neutral competence: Patronage, performance, and policy agreement in executive appointments. Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 25(2), 341–372.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hollibaugh, G. E., Jr. (2015b). Vacancies, vetting, and votes: A unified dynamic model of the appointments process. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 27(2), 206–236.CrossRef Hollibaugh, G. E., Jr. (2015b). Vacancies, vetting, and votes: A unified dynamic model of the appointments process. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 27(2), 206–236.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hollibaugh, G. E., Jr., Horton, G., & Lewis, D. E. (2014). Presidents and patronage. American Journal of Political Science, 58(4), 1024–1042.CrossRef Hollibaugh, G. E., Jr., Horton, G., & Lewis, D. E. (2014). Presidents and patronage. American Journal of Political Science, 58(4), 1024–1042.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hollibaugh, G. E., Jr., Rothenberg, L. S., & Rulison, K. K. (2012). Does it really hurt to be out of step? Political Research Quarterly, 66(4), 856–867.CrossRef Hollibaugh, G. E., Jr., Rothenberg, L. S., & Rulison, K. K. (2012). Does it really hurt to be out of step? Political Research Quarterly, 66(4), 856–867.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hotelling, H. (1929). Stability in competition. The Economic Journal, 39(153), 41–57.CrossRef Hotelling, H. (1929). Stability in competition. The Economic Journal, 39(153), 41–57.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huber, J. D., & McCarty, N. (2004). Bureaucratic capacity, delegation, and political reform. American Political Science Review, 98(3), 481–494.CrossRef Huber, J. D., & McCarty, N. (2004). Bureaucratic capacity, delegation, and political reform. American Political Science Review, 98(3), 481–494.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jessee, S. A. (2012). Ideology and spatial voting in American elections. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Jessee, S. A. (2012). Ideology and spatial voting in American elections. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat John, O. P. (1990). The “Big Five” factor taxonomy: Dimensions of personality in the natural language and in questionnaires. In A. Lawrence, L. Pervin, & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 66–100). New York: Guilford Press. John, O. P. (1990). The “Big Five” factor taxonomy: Dimensions of personality in the natural language and in questionnaires. In A. Lawrence, L. Pervin, & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 66–100). New York: Guilford Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Kinder, D. R., Peters, M. D., Abelson, R. P., & Fiske, S. T. (1980). Presidential prototypes. Political Behavior, 2(4), 315–337.CrossRef Kinder, D. R., Peters, M. D., Abelson, R. P., & Fiske, S. T. (1980). Presidential prototypes. Political Behavior, 2(4), 315–337.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kullback, S., & Leibler, R. A. (1951). On information and sufficiency. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 22(1), 79–86.CrossRef Kullback, S., & Leibler, R. A. (1951). On information and sufficiency. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 22(1), 79–86.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lewis, D. E. (2008). The politics of presidential appointments: Political control and bureaucratic performance. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRef Lewis, D. E. (2008). The politics of presidential appointments: Political control and bureaucratic performance. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lewis, D. E. (2009). Revisiting the administrative presidency: Policy, patronage, and agency competence. Presidential Studies Quarterly, 39(1), 60–73.CrossRef Lewis, D. E. (2009). Revisiting the administrative presidency: Policy, patronage, and agency competence. Presidential Studies Quarterly, 39(1), 60–73.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T, Jr. (1994). The stability of personality: Observations and evaluations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(6), 173–175.CrossRef McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T, Jr. (1994). The stability of personality: Observations and evaluations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(6), 173–175.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McCurley, C., & Mondak, J. J. (1995). Inspected by #1184063113: The influence of incumbents’ competence and integrity in U.S. house elections. American Journal of Political Science, 39(4), 864–885.CrossRef McCurley, C., & Mondak, J. J. (1995). Inspected by #1184063113: The influence of incumbents’ competence and integrity in U.S. house elections. American Journal of Political Science, 39(4), 864–885.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Moe, T. M. (1985). The politicized presidency. In J. E. Chubb & P. E. Peterson (Eds.), The new direction in American politics. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press. Moe, T. M. (1985). The politicized presidency. In J. E. Chubb & P. E. Peterson (Eds.), The new direction in American politics. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Mondak, J. J. (1995). Competence, integrity, and the electoral success of congressional incumbents. Journal of Politics, 57(4), 1043–1069.CrossRef Mondak, J. J. (1995). Competence, integrity, and the electoral success of congressional incumbents. Journal of Politics, 57(4), 1043–1069.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nathan, R. P. (1983). The Administrative Presidency. New York: Wiley. Nathan, R. P. (1983). The Administrative Presidency. New York: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Oh, I.-S., Wang, G., & Mount, M. K. (2011). Validity of observer ratings of the five-factor model of personality traits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(4), 762–773.CrossRef Oh, I.-S., Wang, G., & Mount, M. K. (2011). Validity of observer ratings of the five-factor model of personality traits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(4), 762–773.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ottati, V. C. (1990). Determinants of political judgments: The joint influence of normative and heuristic rules of inference. Political Behavior, 12(2), 159–179.CrossRef Ottati, V. C. (1990). Determinants of political judgments: The joint influence of normative and heuristic rules of inference. Political Behavior, 12(2), 159–179.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Page, B. I. (1994). Democratic responsiveness? Untangling the links between public opinion and policy. PS: Political Science & Politics, 27(1), 25–29. Page, B. I. (1994). Democratic responsiveness? Untangling the links between public opinion and policy. PS: Political Science & Politics, 27(1), 25–29.
Zurück zum Zitat Page, B. I., & Shapiro, R. Y. (1983). Effects of public opinion on policy. American Political Science Review, 77(1), 175–190.CrossRef Page, B. I., & Shapiro, R. Y. (1983). Effects of public opinion on policy. American Political Science Review, 77(1), 175–190.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pateman, C. (1976). Participation and democratic theory. New York: Cambridge University Press. Pateman, C. (1976). Participation and democratic theory. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (1997). Congress: A political-economic history of roll call voting. New York: Oxford University Press. Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (1997). Congress: A political-economic history of roll call voting. New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Ramey, A. (2016). Vox Populi, Vox Dei? Crowdsourced ideal point estimation. Journal of Politics, 78(1), 281–295.CrossRef Ramey, A. (2016). Vox Populi, Vox Dei? Crowdsourced ideal point estimation. Journal of Politics, 78(1), 281–295.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ramey, A. J., Klingler, J. D., & Hollibaugh, G. E., Jr. (2017). More than a feeling: Personality, polarization, and the transformation of the U.S. congress. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef Ramey, A. J., Klingler, J. D., & Hollibaugh, G. E., Jr. (2017). More than a feeling: Personality, polarization, and the transformation of the U.S. congress. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Redlawsk, D. P., & Lau, R. R. (2006). I like him, but\(\ldots \): Vote choice when candidate likeability and closeness on issues clash. In D. P. Redlawsk (Ed.), Feeling politics: Emotion in political information processing (pp. 187–208). New York: Palgrave MacMillan.CrossRef Redlawsk, D. P., & Lau, R. R. (2006). I like him, but\(\ldots \): Vote choice when candidate likeability and closeness on issues clash. In D. P. Redlawsk (Ed.), Feeling politics: Emotion in political information processing (pp. 187–208). New York: Palgrave MacMillan.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The power of personality: The comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4), 313–345.CrossRef Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The power of personality: The comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4), 313–345.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rubenzer, S. J., & Faschingbauer, T. R. (2004). Personality, character, and leadership in the White House: Psychologists assess the presidents. Sterling, VA: Potomac Books Inc. Rubenzer, S. J., & Faschingbauer, T. R. (2004). Personality, character, and leadership in the White House: Psychologists assess the presidents. Sterling, VA: Potomac Books Inc.
Zurück zum Zitat Saward, M. (1998). The terms of democracy. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press. Saward, M. (1998). The terms of democracy. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Shapiro, I. (2009). The state of democratic theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRef Shapiro, I. (2009). The state of democratic theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shor, B., Berry, C., & McCarty, N. (2010). A bridge to somewhere: Mapping state and congressional ideology on a cross-institutional common space. Legislative Studies Quarterly, 35(3), 417–448.CrossRef Shor, B., Berry, C., & McCarty, N. (2010). A bridge to somewhere: Mapping state and congressional ideology on a cross-institutional common space. Legislative Studies Quarterly, 35(3), 417–448.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Soto, C. J., & John, O. P. (2009). Using the California Psychological Inventory to assess the Big Five personality domains: A hierarchical approach. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(1), 25–38.CrossRef Soto, C. J., & John, O. P. (2009). Using the California Psychological Inventory to assess the Big Five personality domains: A hierarchical approach. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(1), 25–38.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stimson, J. A., MacKuen, M. B., & Erikson, R. S. (1995). Dynamic representation. American Political Science Review, 89(3), 543–565.CrossRef Stimson, J. A., MacKuen, M. B., & Erikson, R. S. (1995). Dynamic representation. American Political Science Review, 89(3), 543–565.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stokes, D. E. (1963). Spatial models of party competition. American Political Science Review, 57(2), 368–377.CrossRef Stokes, D. E. (1963). Spatial models of party competition. American Political Science Review, 57(2), 368–377.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tausanovitch, C., & Warshaw, C. (2013). Measuring constituent policy preferences in congress, state legislatures, and cities. Journal of Politics, 75(2), 330–342.CrossRef Tausanovitch, C., & Warshaw, C. (2013). Measuring constituent policy preferences in congress, state legislatures, and cities. Journal of Politics, 75(2), 330–342.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Vecchione, M., Castro, J. L. G., & Caprara, G. V. (2011). Voters and leaders in the mirror of politics: Similarity in personality and voting choice in Italy and Spain. International Journal of Psychology, 46(4), 259–270.CrossRef Vecchione, M., Castro, J. L. G., & Caprara, G. V. (2011). Voters and leaders in the mirror of politics: Similarity in personality and voting choice in Italy and Spain. International Journal of Psychology, 46(4), 259–270.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Vuong, Q. (1989). Likelihood ratio tests for model selection and non-nested hypotheses. Econometrica, 57(2), 307–333.CrossRef Vuong, Q. (1989). Likelihood ratio tests for model selection and non-nested hypotheses. Econometrica, 57(2), 307–333.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
What I Like About You: Legislator Personality and Legislator Approval
verfasst von
Jonathan D. Klingler
Gary E. Hollibaugh Jr.
Adam J. Ramey
Publikationsdatum
10.04.2018
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Political Behavior / Ausgabe 2/2019
Print ISSN: 0190-9320
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6687
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9460-x

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2019

Political Behavior 2/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Premium Partner