Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Cognitive Processing 1/2022

Open Access 09.11.2021 | Research Article

Schoolchildren’s autobiographical memory: COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism effects on emotional content and quality of first memories

verfasst von: Pirko Tõugu, Tiia Tulviste, Toomas Veidebaum, Jaanus Harro

Erschienen in: Cognitive Processing | Ausgabe 1/2022

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Autobiographical memory is a cognitive function strongly related to emotional processing as autobiographical memory often includes emotional content. The COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism is associated with both cognitive and emotional processing. COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism effects on the emotional content and quality of Estonian schoolchildren’s first autobiographical memories were investigated in the present study. In addition, gender effects were considered and the emotional valence of the first memory was taken into account. Schoolchildren’s (N = 234) first memories were coded for valence, emotion words, specificity, and details. Girls were more likely to provide specific memories and recollections with an emotional valence than boys were. Children described memories with a positive or a negative valence in more detail than neutral memories. Interactions between the COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism and gender and valence of the events were detected: Val/Met heterozygotes provided fewer details for emotional events; Val/Met heterozygote boys reported fewer details for their first memories than Val/Met heterozygote girls did; Met/Met homozygote children provided fewer evaluative details for emotional events.
Hinweise
Section Editor: Massimiliano Palmiero (University of Bergamo); Handling Editor: Anouschka Foltz (University of Graz); Reviewers: Jessica Dugan (Emory University) and a second researcher who prefers to remain anonymous.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Autobiographical memory is autonoetic episodic memory constituting a part of the declarative memory (Tulving 2002). It is also a basic human cognitive capacity strongly related to emotional processing, as autobiographical memory often includes emotional content. The COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism has been related to both cognitive and emotional processing; therefore, its role in autobiographical memory formation could be significant. The present study aims at establishing how the COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism is related to the details and emotional content of the first autobiographical memories remembered by Estonian schoolchildren.

First autobiographical memories

Autobiographical memory skills develop alongside other cognitive attainments and are influenced by socialization (e.g., reminiscing with parents) throughout the preschool years (Nelson and Fivush 2004). The cognitive processes for remembering and recalling also become more efficient and effective during childhood (Bauer 2015). Autobiographical memory is most often defined as memory for specific one-time events (Fivush et al. 2011; Tulving 2002), and most people date the first events they can recall to an age of three to four years (see Bauer 2015, for review). The phenomenon of childhood amnesia has led researchers to propose several qualities of early memories that may enhance their survival: Early memories are often described as specific (Mullen 1994) and emotional (Howes et al. 1993).
The majority of people usually report specific events as their first autobiographical memories (Mullen 1994). Specific memories are contrasted to general memories and include at least one particular event that does not last longer than a day. General memories could include descriptions of routine events, or prolonged periods. At the same time, not all first recollections are of one-time specific events. Nuttall et al. (2014) have shown that children’s ability to provide specific one-time events increases with age and on the average only about half of the memories that 4–6-year-olds recall are specific. Studies suggest that memories, especially early memories, tend to become more generalized over the years. The kindergarten memories of school-age children reflect more general experiences than school memories (Picard et al. 2009), and general qualities are prominent in young adult’s very early first recollections (Howes et al. 1993). Women report more specific memories in their life histories compared to men (Pillemer et al. 2003).
Several studies have suggested that the first autobiographical memories are mostly of emotional events (Howes et al. 1993; Kihlstrom and Harackiewicz 1982). At the same time, other studies suggest that affect or emotion is often not explicitly mentioned in recollections (Mullen 1994). Several studies have identified gender differences in reporting emotions in first memories. Women’s recollections include more emotions they experienced during the event (Bauer et al. 2003; Fuentes and Desrocher 2013; Mullen 1994) and use more affective language describing past events (Rice and Pasupathi 2010) as compared to men. All children recall details about their own actions, other people, and artifacts involved in the events (Tõugu et al. 2014). At the same time, girls’ memories include more inner state talk (emotions and cognitions) than those of boys during the preschool years (Fivush et al. 1995) and later (Buckner and Fivush 1998).
Autobiographical memories include episodic and semantic components (Levine et al. 2002). The episodic details refer to the particular one-time-event at a specific place, while semantic details comprise knowledge about the world and oneself in it (Levine et al. 2002). Young children aged 5–6 years usually provide only the core episodic information; 8–9-year-olds report a fuller episodic account of the event and add evaluative information, i.e., they more often include a personal perspective of the event by including a modifier, intensifier (“It was very, very long.”), or by commenting on internal states (“I liked it.”) (Bauer and Larkina 2014). Women have been shown to recollect more details regarding the personal events (MacDonald et al. 2000; Wang 2013; Wang et al. 2011), particularly specific episodic details as compared to general semantic details (Wang et al. 2011). Conflicting findings regarding gender differences have been reported in studies aimed at children’s autobiographical memories. Some studies have shown that the preschool-age girls’ (Fivush et al. 1995; Pasupathi and Wainryb 2010) and women’s (Bauer et al. 2007; Buckner and Fivush 1998) personal narratives are reported in more detail than those of boys and men. Other studies have shown that the recollections of preschool boys and girls do not differ in detail (Wang 2004, 2008) or in episodic information (Tustin and Hayne 2010).
Childhood amnesia is already observable during middle childhood, as during that period children forget earlier events at an exponential rate (Bauer 2015). Children at the age of 8 and 9 begin to display an adult-like distribution of childhood memories (Bauer and Larkina 2014). Bauer suggests that many of the memories that children provide as their first in middle childhood have a potential of being retained as the first memory in adulthood (Bauer 2015; Bauer and Larkina 2014). Hence, middle childhood is a potent period to study the formation of adult-like autobiographical memory. Including genetic markers in the study may also help to shed light on some of the individual differences in autobiographical memory research.

The COMT gene Val158Met functional polymorphism

Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) is an enzyme that participates in the catabolism of catecholamines, including dopamine, and is associated with the functioning of the prefrontal cortex (Tunbridge et al. 2004). The COMT gene contains a functional polymorphism in codon 158, where methionine (Met) can be substituted for valine (Val). The Met form of the COMT enzyme is less active and leads to higher levels of synaptic dopamine in the prefrontal cortex (Chen et al. 2004; Lotta et al. 1995). As the COMT gene is important for modulating dopamine levels, the COMT gene Val158Met functional polymorphism is associated with cognitive functioning and emotion regulation. Mier et al. (2010) have conducted a meta-analysis of fMRI studies that investigated the influence of COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism on prefrontal activation using cognitive processing tasks (working memory and memory processes) and emotional processing (affective stimuli) tasks. The authors of the meta-analysis (Mier et al. 2010) present opposing effects of the COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism on cognitive functioning and emotion processing.
In terms of cognitive processes, they (Mier et al. 2010) show that there is a significant association between the COMT genotype and the prefrontal activation and Met allele carriers show less activation (and enhanced processing). Studies showing Met allele carriers displaying better performance in various cognitive functions are numerous (Barnett et al. 2007, 2008; Bruder et al. 2005; De Frias et al. 2004; Starr et al. 2007). Similarly, the COMT gene has been related to several cognitive functions (Barnett et al. 2007, 2009; Diamond et al. 2004; Dumontheil et al. 2011), including autobiographical memory (Tõugu et al. 2016) in child samples. Preschool children who are Met/Met homozygotes provide more memory details when asked to report on experienced events than children who are Val/Met heterozygotes (Tõugu et al. 2016).
For emotional processing, the meta-analysis by Mier et al. (2010) revealed an association between the COMT genotype and prefrontal activation where carriers of Met alleles show stronger activation and reduced efficiency. Yet, the effect could be more complex. For example, on the one hand, Met alleles were correlated with more difficulties in verbalizing feelings (Swart et al. 2011). On the other hand, the Met allele has been related to faster emotion recognition (Tamm et al. 2016). Barzman et al. (2015) have proposed COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism as one of the possible genetic factors in emotion dysregulation in children and adolescents. And indeed, Val/Val homozygote adolescent girls have reported greater maintenance of positive emotions during stress (Waugh et al. 2009). In the same study, girls with a Val allele also reported greater perceived social acceptance.
At the same time, the genotype effect could be modified by gender or environmental factors. For example, infants with a Val allele who had mothers with a maltreatment history used a more maladaptive emotion regulation strategy in a stressful situation (Villani et al. 2018). Similarly, Val/Val genotype children with a history in institutional care score higher than their Met/Met or Val/Met genotype counterparts on depressive symptoms, especially when they remain in institutional care (as compared to being placed in foster homes) (Drury et al.2010). Also, carriers of the Val allele who perceive higher maternal rejection report higher levels of loneliness compared to Met/Met homozygotes (Wei et al. 2021). These studies show that Val/Val homozygotes could be particularly vulnerable to aversive environmental factors in childhood. In the cognitive domain, the effect of COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism has been shown to differ for the sexes. The gene polymorphism has a notable effect on boys’ cognitive performance, but no effect on girls’ performance (Barnett et al. 2007, 2009).

The present study

The present study sets out to investigate the link between the quality and content of children’s early autobiographical memory and COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism. Autobiographical memory is a cognitive ability. Memory processes affect the formation of personal memories and childhood amnesia (Bauer 2015). At the same time, the quality of autobiographical memories is indicative of psychopathology as people diagnosed with depression often report scant and very general autobiographical memories (Sumner et al. 2010) and struggle with providing positive memories, in particular (Feurer et al. 2018). Understanding how the cognitive and emotional processes contribute to the formation of early memories would broaden our understanding of autobiographical memory and its mechanisms. Comprehension of these mechanisms could also have practical implications for boosting children’s resilience.
The COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism is related to cognitive functions and to early autobiographical memory development. This implies that COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism affects autobiographical memory formation via its influence on the cognitive processes. Children with a Met allele would have qualitatively better early memories to report.
In addition, since first autobiographical memories often include emotional content, the prospective effect of COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism on emotional processing could be apparent in children’s recollections. Since Met alleles are associated with reduced processing efficiency for emotional stimuli, remembering emotional events, in particular, could be enhanced in the group of children with a Val allele.
The present study aims to investigate the effect of COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism on the quality and emotional content of early memories of children. In order to do this, the quality of memory is defined by assessing the specificity of the reported event and calculating the number of reported details (both related to the event and additional evaluative information). The emotional content of the reported memory is explicated by assessing its valence and calculating the number of emotion words used in the report.

Hypotheses

The COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism is expected to influence the valence of the remembered event, the specificity of the memory, the emotional content, and the number of details of children’s autobiographical memories. Based on the previous research, we formulate the hypotheses:
1.
Met/Met homozygotes provide more specific and detailed first autobiographical memories overall than Val/Met heterozygotes and Val/Val homozygotes.
 
2.
Met/Met homozygotes report events with a positive/negative valence less often and are less likely to use emotional words than children with Val/Met or Val/Val genotypes.
 
3.
Girls recall (a) more emotional and specific events than boys and (b) provide more details and (c) more emotional content for their first autobiographical memories than boys.
 
Exploratory analyses are carried out regarding COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism interaction effects by gender and by valence on the event and regarding the effect of valence on the number of details reported.

Method

Participants

All children were participants in the large-scale longitudinal Estonian Children Personality, Behavior, and Health Study (Tomson et al. 2011). The present sample includes children who participated in the additional block devoted to the investigation of autobiographical memory. Data were collected in 2014 in Estonian schools. All participants were Caucasian and attendees of an Estonian-speaking school who had previously attended Estonian-speaking kindergartens. Overall, 321 schoolchildren aged 9–13 (M = 10.2, SD = .77) participated in the additional block: 160 of them were boys and 161 of them girls. Genotype information is available for 245 participating children (Mage = 10.2, SD = .77, 50.6% girls). Due to some missing data, the sample used for the analyses included 234 schoolchildren aged 9–13 (Mage = 10.2, SD = .77), 114 boys and 120 girls.

Procedure and materials

Parents of the children in the longitudinal study were contacted to provide written consent. Children whose parents had provided consent were invited to partake in data collection at school during a lesson in a separate classroom. Participants were asked to fill in a written questionnaire individually. In the instruction of the autobiographical memory task, children were asked to recall the very first memory of their life that they could remember and write it down in as much detail as they could. They were also asked to indicate their age at the time of the remembered event, their age at present, and gender. A research assistant was present to answer questions if the child did not understand the task. The research assistant also made sure that the children did not talk to each other during the task. The task took about 20–25 min to complete.

Genotyping

Blood samples for genotyping were collected from the participants already during the first wave of the study when they were 3–5 years old. Genomic DNA was extracted from dried blood spots using Qiagen QIAamp®DNA Mini Kit (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany). A standard procedure outlined by the manufacturer (Thermo Fisher Scientific 2012) was used for genotyping. Applied Biosystems™ TaqMan® SNP Genotyping Assays use TaqMan® 5´ nuclease chemistry for amplifying and detecting specific polymorphisms in purified genomic DNA samples. Each assay allows genotyping of individuals for a single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP). All assays are developed using bioinformatics assay design processes that apply heuristic rules that are deduced from both manufacturing and assay performance data. Genotyping reactions of COMT Val158Met polymorphism (rs4680) were performed in a total volume of 20 μl with10–50 ng of template DNA as previously described (Lehto et al. 2013). The real-time polymerase chain reaction (RT-PCR) was performed with primers and fluorescent probes obtained from Applied Biosystems (Foster City, CA, USA) Custom TaqMan SNP Genotyping Assays. RT-PCR reaction components and final concentrations were as follows: 1:5 5 × HOT FIREPol Probe qPCR Mix Plus (ROX) (Solis BioDyne) and 1:20 80 × TaqMan Primers Probe (F 5′-CCCAGCGGATGGTGGAT-3′; R 5′-CAGGCATGCACACCTTGTC-3′; Reporter1-TTCGCTGGCATGAAG (VIC); Reporter 2-TCGCTGGCGTGAAG (FAM)). Reactions were performed on the ABI 7500 Real-Time PCR system, and the amplification procedure consisted of an initial denaturation step at 95 °C for 15 min and forty 15-s cycles at 95 °C and 1 min cycles at 60 °C. All genotyping reactions were carried out in duplicates, and extra negative controls were added to each reaction plate. No inconsistencies occurred. Genotypes were found to be in the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium.

Coding

In total, 321 children participated in the autobiographical memory task, but only the memories of children whose genotype information was available (N = 245) were analyzed further. Eleven of them either left the response sheet blank or wrote “I don’t remember.” Overall, 234 first memories of schoolchildren were analyzed. Two coders were trained for each of the coding schemes. The second coder was always the first author. The coder and the author coded 15% of the material in order to calculate intercoder reliability. All discrepancies related to coding the different aspects of the data were resolved in discussion, and the first author proceeded to code the rest of the data.

Memory quality: specificity and details

The memories that the children provided were categorized as specific or general, based on Nuttall et al. (2014). Specific memories (N = 183) described at least one particular event that did not last longer than a day; general memories (N = 28) included descriptions of routine events or prolonged periods, e.g., summer vacations. In addition, memories were coded as semantic memories (N = 23) if children described memories that they did not recall themselves, but what they had been told about, or where they listed life events without describing the event itself. Examples for all the categories are provided in Table 1. Intercoder agreement for coding the specificity of events was 94%.
Table 1
Examples of different types of memory narratives provided by the children
Type of memory
Examples of memory narratives provided
Specific memory
9-year-old girl: When the Christmas tree caught on fire at Christmas because it was too dry for the sparkling candles. Everyone got really upset and was running around with their cups. It happened at home and we didn’t need the fire brigade
11-year-old boy: My best friend gave me his guitar to play with in kindergarten. I played with it and I broke it by accident. I gave it back and said that another boy did it. The other boy got shouted at but I didn’t
General memory
10-year-old boy: I went to visit my aunt in Germany, but all I remember is my niece’s place and even there I only remember the ceiling
11-year-old boy: I was home with my little brother. I played with him
Semantic memory
10-year-old boy: I was sleeping and my uncle put some dice on my head and a book in my hands
11-year-old boy: I was lying on the floor and then my dog Patrick would give me his ears to help me get up. I don’t remember it myself, but my mum and dad tell me, he used to do it all the time
Specific memories were further coded for episodic and extra-episodic details based on the coding scheme of Levine et al. (2002). Episodic details referred to the memory event described and included mentioning actions (“I fell down the tree.”), describing the circumstances of the event (“It was dark.”, “The ship was blue and white.”), and pointing out feelings, thoughts and evaluations (“We were so scared.”, “It was nice.”). Extra-episodic details were details that added semantic information (“I was living in the country-side then.”, “Now the dog has died and we have a new one.”) or provided information about another (often related) event. Intercoder agreement for coding details was 77%.

Emotional content

First, the reported memory events were categorized according to valence either as (1) neutral (memory events where no reference was made to the emotions experienced at the time and the event itself carried no positive or negative implications, e.g., playing with friends, going for an outing, etc.), (2) involving negative emotion (e.g., accidents, being scared or ill, etc.), or (3) involving positive emotion (e.g., birthday celebrations and events when the child indicated being happy or excited) (adapted from Peterson et al. 2005).
Second, all positive emotion words and negative emotion words were counted. Emotion words were words that expressed emotions or emotional actions or provided an emotional evaluation of the event (e.g., positive emotion words included words and expressions like “I was excited.” “We laughed.”, “I loved it.” etc., while examples of negative emotion words include “I cried and yelled.”, “It was horrible.”, “We got scared.” etc.). Intercoder agreement for emotional valence was 80%, and for emotion words 97%. Emotion words were very infrequent in children’s event memories (range 0–6, M = .75, SD = 1.02), and many children (n = 133) did not use any emotion words. In order to use the variable in the analyses, a dichotomous variable was created differentiating children who used at least one emotion word (n = 101) and children who did not use any emotion words (n = 133).

Results

Quality of first memories

The quality of first memories was assessed by specificity of the first memory and the number of details (episodic and extra-episodic) provided in the memory account. One hundred and eighty-three children provided a specific memory, and 51 children described a general memory or semantic memory as their first memory. On average, a specific memory provided by children included 7.52 (SD = 4.91) episodic details and 2.42 (SD = 3.54) extra-episodic details. Descriptive information about the first memories is provided in Tables 2 and 3.
Table 2
Descriptive statistics of children’s first memories for boys and girls with a different COMT Val158Met genotype
Gender
Genotype
% of children providing specific memories
% of children recalling negative or positive events
% of children using emotion words
Number of episodic details
M (SD)
Number of extra-episodic details
M (SD)
Boys
Val/Val (n = 30)
66.7
46.7
36.7
7.55 (4.92)
3.05 (3.36)
Val/Met (n = 52)
75.0
51.9
26.9
5.23 (3.13)
1.15 (1.80)
Met/Met (n = 32)
75.0
56.3
28.1
8.88 (4.53)
1.42 (1.91)
Girls
Val/Val (n = 19)
84.2
68.4
57.9
7.50 (3.54)
4.13 (3.83)
Val/Met (n = 58)
82.8
72.4
56.9
8.69 (6.14)
3.52 (4.79)
Met/Met (n = 43)
83.7
62.8
53.5
7.56 (4.81)
1.86 (3.20)
Table 3
Mean number of episodic and extra-episodic details provided for positive, negative, and neutral events
 
Positive events
M (SD)
Neutral events
M (SD)
Negative events
M (SD)
Number of episodic details
8.42 (5.60)
5.81 (3.54)
8.28 (5.02)
Number of extra-episodic details
3.42 (4.36)
1.61 (2.94)
2.30 (3.13)
In order to investigate the relationship between memory specificity, genotype, and gender (Hypotheses 1 and 3a), a three-way log-linear model including gender (Male/Female), COMT genotype (Met/Met, Val/Met, Val/Val), and memory type (Specific/Not Specific) was estimated. The final model retained the gender × memory-type interaction. The likelihood ratio of this model was χ2(6) = 5.06, p = .54, and the gender × memory-type interaction was significant (χ2 (1) = 4.73; p < .05). The odds ratio indicated that girls were 1.91 times more likely than boys to provide specific memories as their first memories.
In order to investigate the effect of genotype, gender (Hypotheses 1 and 3b), and the effect of the valence of the event (exploratory analysis) on the number of details reported by children, a generalized linear model for Poisson distribution with a log-linear link function was used. First, a model with the number of children’s episodic details as a dependent factor and gender, genotype, and valence of the event as independent factors was estimated. All two-way interactions were included in the model. The overall model was significant (goodness-of-fit deviance/df = 2.58; likelihood ratio χ2(13) = 90.31, p < .001), indicating that the independent variables were systematically related to the dependent variable. In the full model, gender categories did not differ in the contribution to the model, while Val/Met children provided more details than Val/Val children (β = .58, χ2(1) = 9.87, p < .01) and children provided more details for both positive (β = .60, χ2(1) = 10.13, p = .001) and negative events (β = .81, χ2(1) = 6.43, p = .01) compared to neutral events (see descriptive details in Table 3). Regarding interactions, Val/Met genotype boys provided fewer details (β = − .62, χ2(1) = 15.78, p < .001) than other children (see Fig. 1) and Val/Met children reported fewer details for positive (β = − .45, χ2(1) = 5.12, p < .05) and negative events (β = − .61, χ2(1) = 8.75, p < .01) than other children (see Fig. 2). Main effects and interactions are provided in Table 4.
Table 4
Results for the generalized linear models for Poisson distribution with a log-linear link function for the number of episodic details and extra-episodic details provided by children
Main effects and interactions
Model for episodic details
Model for extra-episodic details
Wald χ2
df
p
Wald χ2
df
p
Intercept
4109.45
1
.00
182.71
1
.00
Gender
.98
1
.32
16.67
1
< .001
COMT
7.99
2
.02
24.96
2
< .001
Valence of the event
37.51
2
< .001
14.52
2
< .001
Gender × COMT
32.95
2
< .001
13.08
2
.001
Gender × valence of the event
.82
2
.66
1.87
2
.39
COMT × valence of the event
13.94
4
.01
29.27
4
< .001
All significant results (p < .05) are indicated in bold
Second, a similar model was estimated with extra-episodic details as a dependent variable and gender, genotype (Hypotheses 1 and 3b), and valence of the event (exploratory analysis) as independent factors. All two-way interactions were included in the model. The overall model was significant (goodness-of-fit deviance/df = 3.63; likelihood ratio χ2(13) = 143.24, p < .001), indicating that the independent variables were systematically related to the dependent variable. In the full model, gender categories and COMT categories did not differ in the contribution to the model; children provided more extra-episodic details for both positive (β = .79, χ2(1) = 6.43, p = .01) and negative events (β = 1.20, χ2(1) = 14.43, p < .001) compared to neutral events (see descriptive information in Table 3). Regarding interactions, Val/Met genotype boys provided fewer extra-episodic details (β = − .82, χ2(1) = 10.56, p = .001) than Val/Met genotype girls (see Fig. 3) and Met/Met genotype children reported fewer extra-episodic details for positive (β = − 1.08, χ2(1) = 7.95, p < .01) and negative events (β = -1.38, χ2(1) = 14.45, p < .001) as compared to other children and neutral events (see Fig. 4). The main effects and interactions are provided in Table 4.

Emotional content of first memories

The emotional content was assessed by the general valence of the reported memory and the use of emotion words in the memory account. Neutral events were reported as their first autobiographical memories by 93 children, 76 children recalled negative events, and 65 positive events. At least one emotion word was used in the memory account by 101 children, while 133 children did not use any emotion words. In order to investigate the relationship between emotional valence, genotype, and gender (Hypotheses 2 and 3a), a three-way log-linear model including gender (Male/Female), COMT genotype (Met/Met, Val/Met, Val/Val), and memory valence (Negative/Neutral/Positive) was built. The analysis produced a final model that retained the gender × memory valence interaction. The likelihood ratio of this model was χ2(6) = 12.68, p = .24, and the gender × memory valence interaction was significant (χ2 (2) = 8.18; p < .05). The cross-tabulation showed that 31.7% of girls provide a neutral memory compared to 48.2% of boys doing so and 35% of girls provide a positive memory compared to 20.2% of boys doing so.
In order to investigate the effect of genotype and gender on the use of emotion words by children (Hypotheses 2 and 3c), a three-way log-linear model including gender (Male/Female), COMT genotype (Met/Met, Val/Met, Val/Val), and the use of emotion words (Yes/No) was built. The analysis produced a final model that retained the gender × use of emotion words interaction. The likelihood ratio of this model was χ2(6) = 5.34, p = .50, and the gender × use of emotion words interaction was significant (χ2 (2) = 16.35; p < .001). The odds ratio indicated that boys were 2.9 times less likely than girls to include emotion words in their memory accounts.

Discussion

The present study set out to establish COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism effects on the quality and emotional content of Estonian schoolchildren’s first autobiographical memories. In addition, gender effects were considered and the emotional valence of the first memory was taken into account.

COMT gene effects

The expected effects of COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism regarding the specificity and emotional content (Hypotheses 1 and 2) were not confirmed in the study. Children with a different genotype were equally likely to recall a specific memory as compared to a general memory or a positive or negative memory as compared to a neutral memory, and to use emotion words in their memory reports. One of the reasons that the main effects of the genotype were not revealed could be children’s age. Prior research has indicated that the effects of COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism on cognitive functions are stronger after puberty (Barnett et al. 2007; Gaysina et al. 2013). Therefore, a study with older children could still reveal main effects of genotype on first autobiographical memories. At the same time, other studies have indicated that the COMT genotype effect may be dependent on the environment (Drury et al. 2010, Tõugu et al. 2016) or gender of the child (Barnett et al. 2007, 2009). In the present study, COMT genotype interaction with gender and valence of the event appeared (see discussion below).

Gender effects

The hypothesis regarding gender effects (Hypotheses 3a and c) was to a large extent supported by the data. We found that girls were almost two times more likely than boys to provide specific memories. In addition, girls provided more emotional memories than boys did: They were more likely than boys to report positive events and include emotion words in their memory narratives.
The gender effects revealed in the present study largely corroborate prior findings. Earlier findings show that women are more likely to provide specific memories than men (Pillemer et al. 2003) and that the first memories of women and girls are more emotional than the first memories of boys and men (Buckner and Fivush 1998; Mullen 1994; Rice and Pasupathi 2010). This gender effect could be due to gender-specific autobiographical memory socialization that takes place during the early years (Buckner and Fivush 1998). Socialization seems to have a significant effect on autobiographical memory development as certain socialization patterns lead to earlier and more detailed first memories (Jack et al. 2009). The gender-specific socialization is apparent, for example, when parents’ reminiscing conversations with daughters are more detailed (Reese and Fivush 1993; Reese et al. 1996) and focus more on the emotional content than their conversations with sons (Adams, et al. 1995; Fivush et al. 2000). At the same time, it cannot be ruled out that girls may also notice and/or report emotional aspects of events more readily.
At the same time, the hypothesis about gender differences in the number of details (both episodic and extra-episodic) provided by children (Hypothesis 3b) was not supported. Gender seemed to have a significant main effect in the model, but when all the main effects and interactions were considered together, boys and girls did not differ in the number of details provided. Instead, there were interactions between gender and COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism regarding the number of details of first memories (see discussion below).

Memory details and valence of the event

Regarding exploratory investigations, the quality of first memories in terms of details provided was related to the valence of the experienced event. Children provided more episodic and extra-episodic details when recounting emotional (positive and negative) events as compared to neutral events. An earlier study by Pasupathi and Wainryb (2010) has found that negative memories of children are more detailed than positive ones. That particular study did not include neutral events, but probed for positive and negative memories. Therefore, it is possible that in free recall children’s memories of negative and positive events are similarly detailed as compared to neutral recollections that contain fewer details.
The episodic and extra-episodic details of a memory serve different functions: Episodic details relate to the quality of the memory, while extra-episodic details provide evaluation of the event and personal importance to the teller (Bauer and Larkina 2014) and serve to interpret the memory (Pasupathi and Wainryb 2010). Based on the current findings, it is not clear if children, in fact, remembered more details because the event was emotional or emotional events supply more details to tell (i.e., talk about the emotions and other’s reaction to it, etc.). However, since both episodic and extra-episodic details are more plentiful for emotional memories, it could be both. In the present study, talk about emotions and emotional actions in the memory accounts was rare. This finding seems to render some support to the idea that emotional events are more memorable as children provide more details about them in general. Yet, while children also provide more extra-episodic details about emotional memories, it could be that these memories are more significant to them and make better stories to tell, as there is additional (evaluative and interpretive) information to add.

COMT gene interactions with valence of the event and gender

The COMT gene interaction with gender and valence of the event were significant regarding the number of details (both episodic and extra-episodic) provided by the children. Most of the interactions uncovered involved heterozygote children: Val/Met genotype boys provided fewer details (both episodic and extra-episodic) than girls with the same genotype did, and Val/Met children reported fewer details for positive and negative events compared to other children and neutral events. In addition, Met/Met homozygote children provided fewer extra-episodic details regarding emotional events than other children did.
An earlier study with the same children (when the children were 4 and 6 years old) revealed that both girls and boys with a Val/Met genotype provided fewer details in general when reporting on past events (Tõugu et al. 2016). Now, at the mean age of ten, the effect persisted for emotional memories as compared to neutral ones and for boys as compared to girls with the same genotype. Earlier studies have found that the effect of COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism on cognitive functions is sometimes only observable in the sample of boys (Barnett et al. 2007, 2009). Hence, the genotype by gender interaction could reflect a trajectory of general autobiographical memory development. At the same time, there could be some general cognitive mechanism involved affecting the Val/Met heterozygote group as several birth cohort representative studies have found that COMT heterozygotes have a large gender gap in education (Kurrikoff et al. 2018; Lehto et al. 2013). In both studies, Val/Met heterozygote men have significantly lower educational attainments than Val/Met heterozygote women in Estonia (Kurrikoff et al. 2018; Lehto et al. 2013).
Alternately, Val/Met genotype carriers could be more vulnerable to environmental factors than other genotypes. An earlier study (Tõugu et al. 2016) reported that Val/Met heterozygote children of mothers who did not have a university degree provided fewer details than Met/Met homozygote children of mothers with a similar educational attainment (at the age of 4, and not later). It is possible that Val/Met heterozygote children aged 9–13 are particularly sensitive to the gender typical socialization discussed earlier.
Interestingly, Met/Met homozygote children provided the smallest amount of extra-episodic or additional interpretive information regarding positive and negative events. As the extra-episodic information is often evaluative and marks the significance of the event for the teller, this result could suggest the differences in emotional processing the study set out to find. Mier et al. (2010) have linked the Met allele with reduced efficiency in emotional processing, and Waugh et al. (2009) have reported positive effects of Val/Val genotype on emotional processing in stressful events for adolescent girls. In the present study, Val/Met heterozygote and Val/Val homozygote children provided more extra-episodic information regarding emotional events than Met/Met homozygote children did; the difference is not detected for neutral events. This could indicate that for emotional events, the evaluative and self-significant information is more readily available to children with a Val allele.
The reasons for COMT gene interactions with valence of the event and gender remain to be learnt. Further studies of memory details with environmental variables taken into account are needed in order to unravel the reasons for gender- and valence-related differences. It would be very informative to see what the COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism effect on autobiographical memory would be in the adult sample.

Conclusions

The present study investigated the effects of COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism and gender on the valence, specificity, details, and emotion words of Estonian schoolchildren’s first autobiographical memories. Overall, it appears that the COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism effect on the cognitive and emotional processing involved in autobiographical memory formation is not as clear-cut as implied in laboratory-based studies (see Mier et al. 2010). Regarding the quality and emotional content of autobiographical memories, there are COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism interactions by gender and by the valence of the remembered event. In ecologically valid situations, certain genotypes seem to render the carrier more susceptible to environmental factors (see also Drury et al. 2010; Wei et al. 2021). In the case of autobiographical memory, socialization or gender alongside genetic factors could influence the readiness to notice, incorporate, or report details related to emotional content of events and self-significance of the memory. This provides room for practical implications and opportunities for interventions that could possibly counterbalance unfortunate circumstances. Therefore, more studies are needed in order to determine the mechanisms of autobiographical memory development, to understand how the genotype polymorphism affects further development of autobiographical memory, and to clarify how it relates to the individual differences in autobiographical memory established in adulthood.

Acknowledgements

Research for the article was supported by the Estonian Research Council under Grants PRG1213 and IUT 42-2 and Personal Research Funding under Grant PSG296.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Adams S, Kuebli J, Boyle PA, Fivush R (1995) Gender differences in parent-child conversations about past emotions: A longitudinal investigation. Sex Roles 33(5):309–323 Adams S, Kuebli J, Boyle PA, Fivush R (1995) Gender differences in parent-child conversations about past emotions: A longitudinal investigation. Sex Roles 33(5):309–323
Zurück zum Zitat Barnett JH, Heron J, Ring SM, Golding J, Goldman D, Xu K, Jones PB (2007) Gender-specific effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val108/158Met polymorphism on cognitive function in children. Am J Psychiatry 164(1):142–149PubMed Barnett JH, Heron J, Ring SM, Golding J, Goldman D, Xu K, Jones PB (2007) Gender-specific effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val108/158Met polymorphism on cognitive function in children. Am J Psychiatry 164(1):142–149PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Barnett JH, Scoriels L, Munafò MR (2008) Meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of the catechol-o-methyltransferase gene Val158/108Met polymorphism. Biol Psychiat 64(2):137–144PubMed Barnett JH, Scoriels L, Munafò MR (2008) Meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of the catechol-o-methyltransferase gene Val158/108Met polymorphism. Biol Psychiat 64(2):137–144PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Barnett JH, Heron J, Goldman D, Jones PB, Ke X (2009) Effects of catechol-O-methyltransferase on normal variation in the cognitive function of children. Am J Psychiatry 166(8):909–916PubMedPubMedCentral Barnett JH, Heron J, Goldman D, Jones PB, Ke X (2009) Effects of catechol-O-methyltransferase on normal variation in the cognitive function of children. Am J Psychiatry 166(8):909–916PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Barzman D, Geise C, Lin P-I (2015) Review of the genetic basis of emotion dysregulation in children and adolescents. World J Psychiatry 5(1):112PubMedPubMedCentral Barzman D, Geise C, Lin P-I (2015) Review of the genetic basis of emotion dysregulation in children and adolescents. World J Psychiatry 5(1):112PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Bauer PJ (2015) A complementary processes account of the development of childhood amnesia and a personal past. Psychol Rev 122(2):204PubMed Bauer PJ (2015) A complementary processes account of the development of childhood amnesia and a personal past. Psychol Rev 122(2):204PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bauer PJ, Larkina M (2014) The onset of childhood amnesia in childhood: a prospective investigation of the course and determinants of forgetting of early-life events. Memory 22(8):907–924PubMed Bauer PJ, Larkina M (2014) The onset of childhood amnesia in childhood: a prospective investigation of the course and determinants of forgetting of early-life events. Memory 22(8):907–924PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bauer P, Stennes L, Haight J (2003) Representation of the inner self in autobiography: women’s and men’s use of internal states language in personal narratives. Memory 11(1):27–42PubMed Bauer P, Stennes L, Haight J (2003) Representation of the inner self in autobiography: women’s and men’s use of internal states language in personal narratives. Memory 11(1):27–42PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bauer PJ, Burch MM, Scholin SE, Güler OE (2007) Using cue words to investigate the distribution of autobiographical memories in childhood. Psychol Sci 18(10):910–916PubMed Bauer PJ, Burch MM, Scholin SE, Güler OE (2007) Using cue words to investigate the distribution of autobiographical memories in childhood. Psychol Sci 18(10):910–916PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bruder GE, Keilp JG, Xu H, Shikhman M, Schori E, Gorman JM, Gilliam TC (2005) Catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) genotypes and working memory: Associations with differing cognitive operations. Biol Psychiat 58(11):901–907PubMed Bruder GE, Keilp JG, Xu H, Shikhman M, Schori E, Gorman JM, Gilliam TC (2005) Catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) genotypes and working memory: Associations with differing cognitive operations. Biol Psychiat 58(11):901–907PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Buckner JP, Fivush R (1998) Gender and self in children’s autobiographical narratives. Appl Cogn Psychol 12(4):407–429 Buckner JP, Fivush R (1998) Gender and self in children’s autobiographical narratives. Appl Cogn Psychol 12(4):407–429
Zurück zum Zitat Chen J, Lipska BK, Halim N, Ma QD, Matsumoto M, Melhem S, Kolachana BS, Hyde TM, Herman MM, Apud J et al (2004) Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am J Hum Genet 75(5):807–821PubMedPubMedCentral Chen J, Lipska BK, Halim N, Ma QD, Matsumoto M, Melhem S, Kolachana BS, Hyde TM, Herman MM, Apud J et al (2004) Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am J Hum Genet 75(5):807–821PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat De Frias CM, Annerbrink K, Westberg L, Eriksson E, Adolfsson R, Nilsson L-G (2004) COMT gene polymorphism is associated with declarative memory in adulthood and old age. Behav Genet 34(5):533–539PubMed De Frias CM, Annerbrink K, Westberg L, Eriksson E, Adolfsson R, Nilsson L-G (2004) COMT gene polymorphism is associated with declarative memory in adulthood and old age. Behav Genet 34(5):533–539PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Diamond A, Briand L, Fossella J, Gehlbach L (2004) Genetic and neurochemical modulation of prefrontal cognitive functions in children. Am J Psychiatry 161(1):125–132PubMed Diamond A, Briand L, Fossella J, Gehlbach L (2004) Genetic and neurochemical modulation of prefrontal cognitive functions in children. Am J Psychiatry 161(1):125–132PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Drury SS, Theall KP, Smyke AT, Keats BJ, Egger HL, Nelson CA, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, Zeanah CH (2010) Modification of depression by COMT val158met polymorphism in children exposed to early severe psychosocial deprivation. Child Abuse Negl 34(6):387–395PubMedPubMedCentral Drury SS, Theall KP, Smyke AT, Keats BJ, Egger HL, Nelson CA, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, Zeanah CH (2010) Modification of depression by COMT val158met polymorphism in children exposed to early severe psychosocial deprivation. Child Abuse Negl 34(6):387–395PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Dumontheil I, Roggeman C, Ziermans T, Peyrard-Janvid M, Matsson H, Kere J, Klingberg T (2011) Influence of the COMT genotype on working memory and brain activity changes during development. Biol Psychiat 70(3):222–229PubMed Dumontheil I, Roggeman C, Ziermans T, Peyrard-Janvid M, Matsson H, Kere J, Klingberg T (2011) Influence of the COMT genotype on working memory and brain activity changes during development. Biol Psychiat 70(3):222–229PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Feurer C, Woody ML, Tsypes A, Burkhouse KL, Champagne K, Gibb BE (2018) Episodic life stress and the development of overgeneral autobiographical memory to positive cues in youth. J Abnorm Child Psychol 46(8):1563–1571PubMedPubMedCentral Feurer C, Woody ML, Tsypes A, Burkhouse KL, Champagne K, Gibb BE (2018) Episodic life stress and the development of overgeneral autobiographical memory to positive cues in youth. J Abnorm Child Psychol 46(8):1563–1571PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Fivush R, Haden C, Adam S (1995) Structure and coherence of preschoolers’ personal narratives over time: Implications for childhood amnesia. J Exp Child Psychol 60(1):32–56 Fivush R, Haden C, Adam S (1995) Structure and coherence of preschoolers’ personal narratives over time: Implications for childhood amnesia. J Exp Child Psychol 60(1):32–56
Zurück zum Zitat Fivush R, Brotman MA, Buckner JP, Goodman SH (2000) Gender differences in parent-child emotion narratives. Sex Roles 42(3–4):233–253 Fivush R, Brotman MA, Buckner JP, Goodman SH (2000) Gender differences in parent-child emotion narratives. Sex Roles 42(3–4):233–253
Zurück zum Zitat Fivush R, Habermas T, Waters TEA, Zaman W (2011) The making of autobiographical memory: Intersections of culture, narratives and identity. Int J Psychol 46(5):321–345PubMed Fivush R, Habermas T, Waters TEA, Zaman W (2011) The making of autobiographical memory: Intersections of culture, narratives and identity. Int J Psychol 46(5):321–345PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Fuentes A, Desrocher M (2013) The effects of gender on the retrieval of episodic and semantic components of autobiographical memory. Memory 21(6):619–632PubMed Fuentes A, Desrocher M (2013) The effects of gender on the retrieval of episodic and semantic components of autobiographical memory. Memory 21(6):619–632PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gaysina D, Xu MK, Barnett JH, Croudace TJ, Wong A, Richards M, Jones PB, Group LG (2013) The catechol-O-methyltransferase gene (COMT) and cognitive function from childhood through adolescence. Biol Psychol 92(2):359–364PubMedPubMedCentral Gaysina D, Xu MK, Barnett JH, Croudace TJ, Wong A, Richards M, Jones PB, Group LG (2013) The catechol-O-methyltransferase gene (COMT) and cognitive function from childhood through adolescence. Biol Psychol 92(2):359–364PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Howes M, Siegel M, Brown F (1993) Early childhood memories: accuracy and affect. Cognition 47(2):95–119PubMed Howes M, Siegel M, Brown F (1993) Early childhood memories: accuracy and affect. Cognition 47(2):95–119PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Jack F, MacDonald S, Reese E, Hayne H (2009) Maternal reminiscing style during early childhood predicts the age of adolescents’ earliest memories. Child Dev 80(2):496–505PubMed Jack F, MacDonald S, Reese E, Hayne H (2009) Maternal reminiscing style during early childhood predicts the age of adolescents’ earliest memories. Child Dev 80(2):496–505PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kihlstrom JF, Harackiewicz JM (1982) The earliest recollection: a new survey. J Pers 50(2):134–148 Kihlstrom JF, Harackiewicz JM (1982) The earliest recollection: a new survey. J Pers 50(2):134–148
Zurück zum Zitat Kurrikoff T, Kaarma K, Tooding L-M, Vaht M, Tulviste T, Veidebaum T, Harro J (2018) Association of the COMT Val108/158Met genotype with professional career and education: the Val-allele is more frequent in managers and in enterprising occupations. Personality Individ Differ 121:213–217 Kurrikoff T, Kaarma K, Tooding L-M, Vaht M, Tulviste T, Veidebaum T, Harro J (2018) Association of the COMT Val108/158Met genotype with professional career and education: the Val-allele is more frequent in managers and in enterprising occupations. Personality Individ Differ 121:213–217
Zurück zum Zitat Lehto K, Akkermann K, Parik J, Veidebaum T, Harro J (2013) Effect of COMT Val158Met polymorphism on personality traits and educational attainment in a longitudinal population representative study. Eur Psychiatry 28:492–498PubMed Lehto K, Akkermann K, Parik J, Veidebaum T, Harro J (2013) Effect of COMT Val158Met polymorphism on personality traits and educational attainment in a longitudinal population representative study. Eur Psychiatry 28:492–498PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Levine B, Svoboda E, Hay JF, Winocur G, Moscovitch M (2002) Aging and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychol Aging 17(4):677–689PubMed Levine B, Svoboda E, Hay JF, Winocur G, Moscovitch M (2002) Aging and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychol Aging 17(4):677–689PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Lotta T, Vidgren J, Tilgmann C, Ulmanen I, Melén K, Julkunen I, Taskinen J (1995) Kinetics of human soluble and membrane-bound catechol O-methyltransferase: a revised mechanism and description of the thermolabile variant of the enzyme. Biochemistry 34(13):4202–4210PubMed Lotta T, Vidgren J, Tilgmann C, Ulmanen I, Melén K, Julkunen I, Taskinen J (1995) Kinetics of human soluble and membrane-bound catechol O-methyltransferase: a revised mechanism and description of the thermolabile variant of the enzyme. Biochemistry 34(13):4202–4210PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat MacDonald S, Uesiliana K, Hayne H (2000) Cross-cultural and gender differences in childhood amnesia. Memory 8(6):365–376PubMed MacDonald S, Uesiliana K, Hayne H (2000) Cross-cultural and gender differences in childhood amnesia. Memory 8(6):365–376PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Mier D, Kirsch P, Meyer-Lindenberg A (2010) Neural substrates of pleiotropic action of genetic variation in COMT: a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 15(9):918PubMed Mier D, Kirsch P, Meyer-Lindenberg A (2010) Neural substrates of pleiotropic action of genetic variation in COMT: a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 15(9):918PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Mullen MK (1994) Earliest recollections of childhood: a demographic analysis. Cognition 52(1):55–79PubMed Mullen MK (1994) Earliest recollections of childhood: a demographic analysis. Cognition 52(1):55–79PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson K, Fivush R (2004) The emergence of autobiographical memory: a social cultural developmental theory. Psychol Rev 111(2):486–511PubMed Nelson K, Fivush R (2004) The emergence of autobiographical memory: a social cultural developmental theory. Psychol Rev 111(2):486–511PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Nuttall AK, Valentino K, Comas M, McNeill AT, Stey PC (2014) Autobiographical memory specificity among preschool-aged children. Dev Psychol 50(7):1963–1972PubMed Nuttall AK, Valentino K, Comas M, McNeill AT, Stey PC (2014) Autobiographical memory specificity among preschool-aged children. Dev Psychol 50(7):1963–1972PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pasupathi M, Wainryb C (2010) On telling the whole story: facts and interpretations in autobiographical memory narratives from childhood through midadolescence. Dev Psychol 46(3):735–746PubMed Pasupathi M, Wainryb C (2010) On telling the whole story: facts and interpretations in autobiographical memory narratives from childhood through midadolescence. Dev Psychol 46(3):735–746PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Peterson C, Grant V, Boland L (2005) Childhood amnesia in children and adolescents: their earliest memories. Memory 13(6):622–637PubMed Peterson C, Grant V, Boland L (2005) Childhood amnesia in children and adolescents: their earliest memories. Memory 13(6):622–637PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Picard L, Reffuveille I, Eustache F, Piolino P (2009) Development of autonoetic autobiographical memory in school-age children: genuine age effect or development of basic cognitive abilities? Conscious Cogn 18(4):864–876PubMed Picard L, Reffuveille I, Eustache F, Piolino P (2009) Development of autonoetic autobiographical memory in school-age children: genuine age effect or development of basic cognitive abilities? Conscious Cogn 18(4):864–876PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pillemer D, Wink P, DiDonato T, Sanborn R (2003) Gender differences in autobiographical memory styles of older adults. Memory 11(6):525–532PubMed Pillemer D, Wink P, DiDonato T, Sanborn R (2003) Gender differences in autobiographical memory styles of older adults. Memory 11(6):525–532PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Reese E, Fivush R (1993) Parental styles of talking about the past. Dev Psychol 29(3):596–606 Reese E, Fivush R (1993) Parental styles of talking about the past. Dev Psychol 29(3):596–606
Zurück zum Zitat Reese E, Haden CA, Fivush R (1996) Mothers, fathers, daughters, sons: gender differences in autobiographical reminiscing. Res Lang Soc Interact 29(1):27–56 Reese E, Haden CA, Fivush R (1996) Mothers, fathers, daughters, sons: gender differences in autobiographical reminiscing. Res Lang Soc Interact 29(1):27–56
Zurück zum Zitat Rice C, Pasupathi M (2010) Reflecting on self-relevant experiences: adult age differences. Dev Psychol 46(2):479PubMed Rice C, Pasupathi M (2010) Reflecting on self-relevant experiences: adult age differences. Dev Psychol 46(2):479PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Starr JM, Fox H, Harris SE, Deary IJ, Whalley LJ (2007) COMT genotype and cognitive ability: a longitudinal aging study. Neurosci Lett 421(1):57–61PubMed Starr JM, Fox H, Harris SE, Deary IJ, Whalley LJ (2007) COMT genotype and cognitive ability: a longitudinal aging study. Neurosci Lett 421(1):57–61PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Sumner JA, Griffith JW, Mineka S (2010) Overgeneral autobiographical memory as a predictor of the course of depression: a meta-analysis. Behav Res Ther 48(7):614–625PubMedPubMedCentral Sumner JA, Griffith JW, Mineka S (2010) Overgeneral autobiographical memory as a predictor of the course of depression: a meta-analysis. Behav Res Ther 48(7):614–625PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Swart M, Bruggeman R, Larøi F, Alizadeh BZ, Kema I, Kortekaas R, Wiersma D, Aleman A (2011) COMT Val158Met polymorphism, verbalizing of emotion and activation of affective brain systems. Neuroimage 55(1):338–344PubMed Swart M, Bruggeman R, Larøi F, Alizadeh BZ, Kema I, Kortekaas R, Wiersma D, Aleman A (2011) COMT Val158Met polymorphism, verbalizing of emotion and activation of affective brain systems. Neuroimage 55(1):338–344PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Tamm G, Kreegipuu K, Harro J (2016) Perception of emotion in facial stimuli: the interaction of ADRA2A and COMT genotypes, and sex. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 64:87–95PubMed Tamm G, Kreegipuu K, Harro J (2016) Perception of emotion in facial stimuli: the interaction of ADRA2A and COMT genotypes, and sex. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 64:87–95PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Tomson K, Merenäkk L, Loit HM, Mäestu J, Harro J (2011) The relationship between serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism and serum lipid levels at young age in a longitudinal population-representative study. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 35(8):1857–1862 Tomson K, Merenäkk L, Loit HM, Mäestu J, Harro J (2011) The relationship between serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism and serum lipid levels at young age in a longitudinal population-representative study. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 35(8):1857–1862
Zurück zum Zitat Tõugu P, Tulviste T, Suits K (2014) Gender differences in the content of preschool children’s recollections: a longitudinal study. Int J Behav Dev 38(6):563–569 Tõugu P, Tulviste T, Suits K (2014) Gender differences in the content of preschool children’s recollections: a longitudinal study. Int J Behav Dev 38(6):563–569
Zurück zum Zitat Tõugu P, Vaht M, Tulviste T, Veidebaum T, Harro J (2016) The association between the COMT gene Val 158 Met polymorphism and preschoolers’ autobiographical memory details and narrative cohesiveness. Cogn Dev 39:181–188 Tõugu P, Vaht M, Tulviste T, Veidebaum T, Harro J (2016) The association between the COMT gene Val 158 Met polymorphism and preschoolers’ autobiographical memory details and narrative cohesiveness. Cogn Dev 39:181–188
Zurück zum Zitat Tulving E (2002) Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol 53(1):1–25PubMed Tulving E (2002) Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol 53(1):1–25PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Tunbridge EM, Bannerman DM, Sharp T, Harrison PJ (2004) Catechol-O-methyltransferase inhibition improves set- shifting performance and elevates stimulated dopamine release in the rat prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 24(23):5331–5335PubMedPubMedCentral Tunbridge EM, Bannerman DM, Sharp T, Harrison PJ (2004) Catechol-O-methyltransferase inhibition improves set- shifting performance and elevates stimulated dopamine release in the rat prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 24(23):5331–5335PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Tustin K, Hayne H (2010) Defining the boundary: Age-related changes in childhood amnesia. Dev Psychol 46(5):1049PubMed Tustin K, Hayne H (2010) Defining the boundary: Age-related changes in childhood amnesia. Dev Psychol 46(5):1049PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Villani V, Ludmer J, Gonzalez A, Levitan R, Kennedy J, Masellis M, Basile VS, Wekerle C, Atkinson L (2018) Dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2), dopamine transporter solute carrier family C6, member 4 (SLC6A3), and catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) genes as moderators of the relation between maternal history of maltreatment and infant emotion regulation. Dev Psychopathol 30(2):581–592PubMed Villani V, Ludmer J, Gonzalez A, Levitan R, Kennedy J, Masellis M, Basile VS, Wekerle C, Atkinson L (2018) Dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2), dopamine transporter solute carrier family C6, member 4 (SLC6A3), and catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) genes as moderators of the relation between maternal history of maltreatment and infant emotion regulation. Dev Psychopathol 30(2):581–592PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wang Q (2004) The emergence of cultural self-constructs: Autobiographical memory and self-descriptions in European American and Chinese children. Dev Psychol 40(1):3–15PubMed Wang Q (2004) The emergence of cultural self-constructs: Autobiographical memory and self-descriptions in European American and Chinese children. Dev Psychol 40(1):3–15PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wang Q (2008) Emotion knowledge and autobiographical memory across the preschool years: a cross-cultural longitudinal investigation. Cognition 108(1):117–135PubMed Wang Q (2008) Emotion knowledge and autobiographical memory across the preschool years: a cross-cultural longitudinal investigation. Cognition 108(1):117–135PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wang Q (2013) Gender and emotion in everyday event memory. Memory 21(4):503–511PubMed Wang Q (2013) Gender and emotion in everyday event memory. Memory 21(4):503–511PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wang Q, Hou Y, Tang H, Wiprovnick A (2011) Travelling backwards and forwards in time: Culture and gender in the episodic specificity of past and future events. Memory 19(1):103–109PubMed Wang Q, Hou Y, Tang H, Wiprovnick A (2011) Travelling backwards and forwards in time: Culture and gender in the episodic specificity of past and future events. Memory 19(1):103–109PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Waugh CE, Dearing KF, Joormann J, Gotlib IH (2009) Association between the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism and self-perceived social acceptance in adolescent girls. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 19(4):395–401PubMedPubMedCentral Waugh CE, Dearing KF, Joormann J, Gotlib IH (2009) Association between the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism and self-perceived social acceptance in adolescent girls. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 19(4):395–401PubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Schoolchildren’s autobiographical memory: COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism effects on emotional content and quality of first memories
verfasst von
Pirko Tõugu
Tiia Tulviste
Toomas Veidebaum
Jaanus Harro
Publikationsdatum
09.11.2021
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Cognitive Processing / Ausgabe 1/2022
Print ISSN: 1612-4782
Elektronische ISSN: 1612-4790
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-021-01064-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2022

Cognitive Processing 1/2022 Zur Ausgabe

Neuer Inhalt