Skip to main content

Open Access 10.05.2024 | Original Article

When We Are Happy, We Are the Same—Emotions as a Boundary Condition for the Impact of Cultural Differences on Strategic Decisions

verfasst von: Sophie Florian, Philip Meissner, Torsten Wulf, Xian Xu, Philip Yang

Erschienen in: Schmalenbach Journal of Business Research

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Strategic decision-making research has mainly relied on the values-based approach to culture. However, the dynamic constructivist approach to culture has shown that cultural tendencies may also be altered by contingency factors in the decision-making process itself. We theorize based on the appraisal tendency framework as well as the concept of cultural affordances that emotions, such as happiness, can alter the cultural dispositions of managers from Western and East Asian contexts. To test our hypotheses, we conduct a lab-in-the-field experiment with 187 executives from China and Germany, and measure emotions based on participants’ psychophysiological skin conductance responses. Our results show that happiness moderates and can even reverse initial cultural dispositions in executives’ strategic decision-making behavior. These findings suggest that emotions may be important contingency factors that can alter the initial cultural dispositions of decision makers in the strategy process.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

1 Introduction

Cultural differences have received increasing research attention as drivers of strategic decision making (Bachmann et al. 2016) and strategic decision outcomes in organizations (Li et al. 2001). Research has shown, for example, that cultural differences affect decisions on corporate diversification (Savitz et al. 2016; Song et al. 2002), the decentralization and formalization of the strategic decision-making process (Dimitratos et al. 2011), and the interpretation of strategic issues (Barr and Glynn 2004).
The extant research on the impact of cultural differences on strategic decision making in organizations is almost exclusively centered on the values-based approach to culture, which differentiates cultures using Hofstede’s (1980) value dimensions. However, this approach has been criticized in the extant literature (Tung and Verbeke 2010; Oyserman and Lee 2008; Shenkar 2001), because it tends to regard cultural dispositions as static, while research findings suggest that the effects of cultural differences might also be context-dependent (Chiu et al. 2000).
Psychological research on culture and decision making has introduced a dynamic constructivist approach to culture (Hong and Chiu 2001), which describes culture as a system of knowledge structures or folk theories (Peng and Knowles 2003; Ji 2005) that result from long-established cultural differences in social organization and social practices (Nisbett et al. 2001). Such differences manifest themselves in, for instance, different cognitive styles, which have been observed in East Asian and Western managers (Li et al. 2018; Varnum et al. 2010) and affect their strategic decision-making behavior (Tsui et al. 2007). The dynamic constructivist approach suggests that decision makers do not always display the behavioral tendencies associated with their cultural backgrounds. Rather, a specific behavioral effect of culture may change if the behavioral context changes (Chiu et al. 2000). Research has, for example, identified cognitive priming (Hong et al. 2000) and experience in two cultures (Morris and Fu 2001) as boundary conditions for changes in the behavioral effects of cultural differences.
Psychological research on culture and decision making indicates that emotions might also constitute a boundary condition that changes the effects of cultural differences on strategic decision making. Specifically, research on appraisal tendencies (Lerner and Keltner 2001) as well as on cultural affordances (Kitayama et al. 2006) suggests that happiness may serve as a contingency factor that changes cultural dispositions. While initial studies in Western cultural settings find that happiness can foster open information processing (Bramesfeld and Gasper 2008; Estrada et al. 1997; Isen et al. 1991; Staw and Barsade 1993), research in an Asian cultural context has shown that happiness leads to a reduction in holistic cognitive tendencies (Au et al. 2003). These findings indicate that emotions, such as happiness, might change the effects of cultural differences between Western and East Asian managers on their strategic decision-making behavior.
In this paper, based on the appraisal tendency framework as well as the concept of cultural affordances, we investigate the role of happiness as a moderator of cross-cultural differences in strategic decision-making behavior. Specifically, we suggest that happiness is an important contingency factor in the strategic decision-making process, and that it can alter the initial cultural dispositions of decision makers from Western and East Asian countries. In line with prior research, we argue that managers from Western and East Asian cultures differ in their strategic decision-making behavior because of the different cognitive styles associated with their respective cultures (Li et al. 2018; Varnum et al. 2010). However, if induced with happiness, we hypothesize that the effect of culture on strategic decision making changes in opposite directions, such that the differences in strategic decision-making behavior between Western and Asian managers vanish. We argue that happiness induces different energizing effects in East Asian and Western managers. For East Asians, the energizing effect of happiness leads to more superficial processing and overconfidence (Au et al. 2003). Western managers, in contrast, are energized to dive more deeply into decision tasks, which triggers a broader and more thorough information search (Staw and Barsade 1993; Isen and Baron 1991). We test our hypotheses using a lab-in-the-field experiment (Koudstaal et al. 2016) involving 187 Chinese and German executives in four randomized groups. We measure emotions based on the psychophysiological skin conductance responses (SCR) of participants (Døjbak Håkonsson et al. 2016). The results of our empirical analysis support our hypotheses.
Our research contributes to the literature on strategic decision making in cross-cultural contexts. Specifically, we make two theoretical contributions. First, we expand our knowledge on the role of cross-cultural differences in strategic decision processes (Dimitratos et al. 2011) by introducing the dynamic constructivist approach to culture into strategic decision-making research. We show that the effects of cultural differences on strategic decision making are not static, but might change based on contingency factors. In addition, we introduce emotions, especially happiness, as a contingency factor that might serve as a boundary condition for the effect of culture on strategic decision making. Thus, we highlight that the effect of culture on strategic decision making is more complex than prior research might suggest.
Second, we contribute to research on the relationship between cognition and emotion by highlighting the role that intercultural differences might play in this relationship. Specifically, we show that the cognitive effects of emotions, such as happiness, differ for managers with different cultural backgrounds. Thus, our findings indicate that future research on the relationship between cognition and emotion should take cultural differences into account (Forgas and George 2001; George and Dane 2016).

2 Theoretical Background

2.1 Cultural Differences and Strategic Decision-making

The effects of cultural differences have been widely studied in the domains of international management (for a review, see López-Duarte et al. 2016), organizational behavior (for a review, see Tsui et al. 2007), and strategic management (e.g., Nielsen and Nielsen 2011; Song et al. 2002). Especially in the context of strategic decision making (i.e., decisions that are characterized by high levels of complexity, uncertainty, and ambiguity; Schwenk 1995), cultural differences affect executives’ choices (Dimitratos et al. 2011) and firm performance (Li et al. 2001).
Most research that analyzes the effect of cross-cultural differences on strategic decisions is rooted in the values-based approach to culture (Li et al. 2017). In other words, it distinguishes among national cultures based on Hofstede’s (1980) value dimensions and compares strategic decisions among members of different cultural groups. This research has shown, for example, that firms in countries with higher levels of uncertainty avoidance, collectivism, and power distance tend to exhibit more corporate diversification (Savitz et al. 2016). Bachmann et al. (2016) find that certain cultural values—especially higher uncertainty avoidance and lower levels of individualism and masculinity, as assessed on the country level in nine countries—moderate the positive effect of strategic planning on the firm’s entrepreneurial orientation. Dimitratos et al. (2011) analyze the impact of cultural values on the strategic decision-making process in firms from four countries and find a negative association between power distance and decentralization as well as positive relationships between individualism and lateral communication and between uncertainty avoidance and rule formalization in the strategic decision-making process. In a study of top managers from 20 countries, Geletkanycz (1997) shows that individualism, power distance, uncertainty avoidance, and long-term orientation are associated with openness to change. However, for two of the four cultural values, the relationship is the reverse of the anticipated direction. Barr and Glynn (2004) attribute differences in executives’ strategic-issue interpretation to differences in uncertainty avoidance in their countries of origin. Specifically, they suggest that executives from countries characterized by high uncertainty avoidance tend to perceive strategic issues as less controllable. However, they do not find any effects for other cultural values.
Overall, research that analyzes the impact of national cultures on strategic decisions based on Hofstede’s (1980) values-based approach has been criticized, as it cannot comprehensively explain the effects of cultural differences observed in the extant research (Tung and Verbeke 2010; Oyserman and Lee 2008; Shenkar 2001). In particular, the values-based approach assumes that culture is defined by fixed dispositions (i.e., static cultural values that hardly change over time; Beugelsdijk et al. 2015). However, inconclusive findings on the effects of cultural differences on strategic decisions suggest that such effects are—at least in part—context-dependent (i.e., when the strategic context changes, the impact of culture on a strategic decision might change; Chiu et al. 2000).
To account for these deficits in our extant knowledge, psychological research on culture and decision making has suggested a dynamic constructivist approach to culture (Hong and Chiu 2001). This approach describes culture as a loose network of knowledge structures and folk theories that result from long-established differences in social organization and social practices, and proposes that these structures mediate social behavior (Hong 2009). Cultural differences manifest themselves in, for example, the social norms, values, and cognitive styles that are shared among the members of a cultural group and guide their decision-making behavior (e.g., Yates and de Oliveira 2016). For instance, when making decisions, Westerners have been found to rely on a more analytical thinking style that is more object-focused and based on the assumptions that the world is stable and predictable (Ji et al. 2008; Miyamoto et al. 2006). East Asians, in contrast, are more likely to apply a more holistic thinking style, rooted in the assumptions that contradictions can coexist and that change is a constant phenomenon (Ji 2005; Li et al. 2014).
Notably, the dynamic constructivist approach proposes that cultural dispositions do not always affect the behavior and decision making of members of a cultural group, and that these effects are not always of the same magnitude. In addition, it suggests that cultural norms, values, and cognitive styles are stored in the memory of the members of a cultural group, where they become available and accessible as part of an interconnected, associative cognitive network (Briley et al. 2014). Whether a member of a cultural group applies these norms, values, and cognitive styles in a specific situation depends on their perceived relevance to the focal task, i.e. the subjective assessment of their applicability in the specific context (Hong 2009). This implies that the effects of cultural norms, values, and cognitive styles on individual behavior in general and decision making in particular may surface, disappear, or even reverse if the behavioral context changes (Chiu et al. 2000).
Research has identified several boundary conditions that determine when certain cultural norms, values, and cognitive styles are activated and affect the behavior and decision making of members of a cultural group. Hong et al. (2000) show that activation can be achieved through cognitive priming based on culture-related stimuli, such as pictures of cultural symbols. In an experiment involving French and Korean consumers, Choi (2020) finds that (independent versus interdependent) self-construal priming reduces the cultural differences between members of these two cultural groups. Briley, Morris, and Simonson (2000) show that East Asian consumers are more willing to accept compromises when they are asked to provide reasons for their choices, while North American participants prefer compromises to a lesser extent when they are forced to provide such reasons. These authors argue that the need to justify a choice activates the more holistic cognitive style that is inherent in East Asian cultures, while in North Americans it brings the more analytical thinking style that characterizes Western cultures to the surface. While extant research adopting the dynamic constructivist approach to culture focuses on cognitive drivers of the activation of cultural norms, values, and cognitive styles, psychological research on culture and decision making suggests that emotions might also serve as a boundary condition for the impact of culture on individual behavior and decision making (Kitayama et al. 2006).

2.2 Emotions and Cultural Differences in Strategic Decision-making

Based on psychological foundations, researchers in the strategic decision-making field have started to explore the effect of emotions on strategic decisions (e.g., Døjbak Håkonsson et al. 2016; Hodgkinson and Healey 2011; Huy 2011). Emotions are defined as discrete, intense, and short-lived adaptive responses to environmental demands (Scherer and Ekman 1984; Elfenbein 2007). Initially, research on emotions in the strategic decision-making context has focused on valence-based conceptualizations of emotions, i.e. on positive vs. negative emotions. For example, Døjbak Håkonsson et al. (2016) have associated positive emotions with more explorative strategies, while Amabile et al. (2005) have found a positive effect of such emotions on creativity in the workplace. Negative emotions, in contrast, have been linked to insensitive communication and problem avoidance (Maitlis and Ozcelik 2004).
Based in particular on the appraisal tendency framework (Lerner and Keltner 2001), more recent research in the strategic decision-making field has started to analyze the effects of discrete emotions, such as anger, fear, and happiness, on the strategic decision-making process (e.g., Meissner et al. 2021; Neumann and Wulf 2022). The appraisal tendency framework posits that discrete emotions trigger specific motivational and cognitive processes—so-called appraisal tendencies—that determine how that emotion impacts judgement and decision-making (Han et al. 2007). For example, happiness has been associated with appraisals of elevated certainty and individual control (Smith and Ellsworth 1985), which—in a Western context—has been linked to more optimistic assessments of risk (Lerner and Keltner 2001) and more heuristic information processing (Forgas 1998).
However, psychological research on culture and emotions indicates that the appraisal tendencies, that are associated with specific emotions, might differ across cultures due to specific cultural affordances (Kitayama and Marcus 1999). The concept of cultural affordances reflects folk theories, social practices, and schemas, that have been shared within a cultural group over generations (Kitayama et al. 2007) and that provide specific meanings to emotional experiences, thus triggering different appraisal tendencies for the same emotion across cultures (Kitayama et al. 2006). For example, individuals from East Asian cultures have been found to experience socially engaging emotions, such as pride, more strongly than Westerners, who experience disengaging emotions, such as anger, to a stronger extent (Kitayama et al. 2006). Matsumoto et al. (2008) find a particularly strong effect of cultural affordances for high-activation, positive emotions such as happiness.
These arguments are supported by findings in the strategic decision-making field, where research has shown that emotions affect cognition and thinking styles (Hodgkinson and Healey 2008, 2011), information processing (Seo and Barrett 2007; Staw and Barsade 1993), and the perception and interpretation of strategic issues (Mittal and Ross 1998). Neumann and Wulf (2022) report that emotions, such as fear and happiness, impact the strategic issue interpretation—as one element of the strategic decision-making process—of German and Chinese managers in different ways. They find that Chinese managers are affected in their interpretation of strategic issues by happiness as well as fear while German managers are only affected by fear. Overall, research in the strategic decision-making field indicates that emotions, such as happiness, might change the effects of cultural differences on the strategic decision-making behavior of Western and East Asian managers.

3 Hypotheses Development

3.1 Cultural Differences in Strategic Decision-making Between Chinese and German Executives

Psychological research on culture and decision making has highlighted differences in the decision-making behavior of East Asian and Western decision makers. For example, Chinese executives have been found to exhibit a more holistic thinking style, which acknowledges the existence of contradictions and of change as a constant phenomenon (Ji 2005; Li et al. 2014), while German decision makers tend to adopt a more object- and stability-focused analytical cognitive style (Ji et al. 2008; Miyamoto et al. 2006).
Furthermore, Chinese decision makers process significantly more information and do so in a more integrated manner (Kühnen and Oyserman 2002; Oyserman and Lee 2008). At the same time, they tend to be less likely to ignore important pieces of information (Choi et al. 2003). On the other hand, Western decision makers typically rely on heuristics when processing information, which results in less attention to detail (Buchtel and Norenzayan 2008; Nisbett et al. 2001). Therefore, in decision-making processes, Western managers are more likely to selectively focus on important information, while more holistic, East Asian executives tend to engage in broader information processing (Li et al. 2015).
Consequently, Chinese executives consider a wider range of factors as relevant in decision situations (Li et al. 2016; Ng and Hynie 2014) and develop a broader set of alternative arguments when making strategic decisions (Kühnen and Oyserman 2002; Oyserman and Lee 2008). For this reason, East Asian decision makers tend to allocate resources more broadly than their Western counterparts (Li et al. 2018). Given the above arguments, we propose that when asked to choose among a number of strategic investment alternatives, Chinese executives will find positive and negative aspects in all options at hand, resulting in a more diversified allocation of strategic resources, while the opposite holds true for German decision makers. We therefore posit:
Hypothesis 1
In a neutral emotional state, Chinese decision makers (German decision makers) make more diversified (more focused) strategic investment decisions.

3.2 The Moderating Role of Happiness on Cultural Differences in Strategic Decision-making

Happiness is an emotion that is frequently experienced in the context of strategic decision making (Hermalin and Isen 2000). Psychological research on emotions and decision making indicates that specific emotions are associated with different appraisal tendencies (Lerner and Keltner 2001) and that these appraisal tendencies might change due to cultural affordances (Kitayama et al. 2006). Specifically, positive emotions, such as happiness, have been linked to changes in information processing in the context of strategic decisions (Staw and Barsade 1993). Initial findings from psychological studies in Western and East Asian contexts have highlighted how analytical (Western) and holistic (East Asian) thinking styles might change in a happy emotional state (Au et al. 2003; Matsumoto and Wilson 2008).
In a study in an East Asian context, rather than exhibiting holistic strategic decision-making behavior, participants in a happy emotional state engaged in more selective information processing, and adopted a more superficial and quick processing style than participants in a control group with a neutral emotional state (Au et al. 2003). Au et al. (2003) attribute this reversal of cultural disposition to the energizing effect of happiness, which induces overconfidence as well as a higher propensity to simplify, to prematurely rely on heuristics, and to take risks (Schwarz and Clore 1983; Wyer et al. 1999).
Similarly, decision makers in a Western context have been shown to deviate from their cultural disposition when in a happy emotional state. In a study of diagnostic decisions in a Western cultural setting, Isen et al. (1991) find that happy decision makers processed a broader, more complex set of information than a control group. Staw and Barsade (1993) show that happy decision makers tend to more broadly support their judgments with data and achieve higher decision quality. These findings are attributed to the energizing effect of happiness that leads Western managers to dive more deeply into decision tasks, which triggers a broader, more thorough information search (Staw and Barsade 1993; Isen and Baron 1991). Similar results have been reported in group settings (Bramesfeld and Gasper 2008).
Based on these findings, we argue that happiness changes the strategic investment behavior of Chinese and German executives in different ways. Specifically, Chinese executives asked to choose among a number of strategic investment alternatives become more selective and focused, which results in more focused strategic investments. In contrast, German executives become likely to adopt a broader, more integrative approach to their information search, which results in greater diversification of their strategic investments. Overall, we predict that happiness—because of its opposite effects on information processing and risk-taking in different cultural contexts—moderates the relationship between culture and investment allocation in a such way that differences in decision-making behavior are reduced or even disappear. Hence, we posit:
Hypothesis 2a
In a happy emotional state, Chinese decision makers make more focused strategic investment decisions than Chinese decision makers in a neutral emotional state.
Hypothesis 2b
In a happy emotional state, German decision makers make more diversified strategic investment decisions than German decision makers in a neutral emotional state.
Hypothesis 2c
Happiness moderates the effect of cultural differences between Chinese and German executives on their strategic investments, such that, in a happy state, the differences in strategic investments between German and Chinese decision makers diminish.

4 Method

4.1 Participants

To test our hypotheses, we conducted a lab-in-the-field experiment involving 97 Chinese and 90 German executives who were participants in executive programs at one Chinese and one German university, respectively. We selected Chinese and Germans, as they have been shown to be representative of East Asian and Western cultures (Yates and de Oliveira 2016). Bicultural individuals were excluded from the experiments to avoid biased results, as they may exhibit divergent choice behaviors based on their two cultures (Mok and Morris 2010; Wang 2008). Overall, the sample consisted of executives between 22 and 65 years of age with 2 to 44 years of work experience. Of the executives, 59% held general management positions, 10% were top managers, and 31% occupied specialized management positions.

4.2 Experimental Design and Procedure

Design. In line with prior research in the fields of strategy (Croson et al. 2007) and management (Koudstaal et al. 2016), we conducted a lab-in-the-field experiment, which combines the advantages of field studies with the controlled setup of laboratory studies (Schwenk 1982). We used a post-test-only control group design in which we randomly assigned participants to either the treatment or the control condition. To avoid social desirability bias, we did not inform participants about the goal of the research prior to the experiment (Grimm 2010).
We conducted the experiment in person with an experimenter present for the duration of the study. At the beginning of the experiment, participants in the treatment group were asked to watch a movie clip intended to induce happiness. Movie clips are frequently used in emotion research (Bramesfeld and Gasper 2008; Fredrickson and Branigan 2005; Lerner and Keltner 2001) and seem particularly effective for inducing emotions (Lazar and Pearlman-Avnion 2014). The movie clip that we used to induce happiness showed a dance scene from the movie 500 Days of Summer. Subjects in the neutral condition were presented with a neutral movie clip of a scene in which a sheet of paper is held out of a car window and is blowing in the wind. Both movie clips have been suggested by Schaefer et al. (2010). They include almost no dialogue, which helps prevent language priming and barriers (Lindquist et al. 2006). To further test the validity of this emotion-induction method, we conducted a pre-study in China and Germany, which confirmed that the videos were effective for eliciting a neutral emotional state (control group) and a happy emotional state (happy group), as intended.
The treatment group included 93 subjects (48 Chinese and 45 German), while 94 participants were randomly assigned to the control group (49 Chinese, 45 German). After watching the emotion-inducing movie clip, participants moved on to an in-basket exercise in which we tested their strategic investment decisions. All questions as well as the case study used in the experiment were translated from English into German and Chinese using a back-translation procedure. This method has frequently been applied in cross-cultural studies and has been shown to produce highly valid results (Brislin 1970; Lechuga and Wiebe 2011; van de Vijver & Leung 1997).
Procedure. We started our experiment with the same introduction for the treatment and control groups. After the introduction, all participants completed a short questionnaire that focused on the control variables used in the study. For our in-basket exercise, participants were then presented with a hypothetical business situation and asked to work through a series of documents, memos, and emails before making a final decision (Kesselman et al. 1982). By providing participants with a number of informational items, in-basket tasks allow for the study of managers’ abilities to handle complexity and ambiguity. Thus, in-basket tasks simulate the ill-structured problems that strategic decision makers face in their daily work (Woodside et al. 2016). Given their rich context, in-basket tasks are regarded as particularly well suited for studies of strategic decision making (Tse et al. 1988; Woodside et al. 2016). As such, they have frequently been utilized in research on decision making (Connelly et al. 2004; Estrada et al. 1997, 1994; Staw and Barsade 1993; Tse et al. 1988).
For our experiment, we used the in-basket case study “Adam & Smith Inc.” (A&S) which was initially developed by Staw (1976) and later expanded by Tosi et al. (2003). The case first provides participants with a short description of the situation facing A&S, a large international manufacturing company that has been experiencing declining profits. In the second step, each participant is asked to act as the head of business development for A&S. He or she is presented with several memos and emails, and with financial information showing that the board of directors has identified a lack of research and development (R&D) efforts as one of the main reasons for the company’s recent decline in profits. Finally, after reviewing all information, each participant is asked to decide how to allocate an additional R&D budget of USD 20 million among the company’s three divisions. Participants are advised to make the allocation decision based on the projected absolute cumulative profitability per division. In this case, the Medical Industrial Products Division shows the highest projected cumulative profits (see Appendices A and B for further details).

5 Measures

Happiness. In line with the circumplex model, we used two separate dimensions—valence and arousal—to measure happiness (Russell 1980). Arousal describes the level of activation that is caused by an environmental stimulus which can range from low (calm) to high (excited), while valence captures the level of pleasantness of an environmental stimulus, ranging from negative to positive (Gomes et al. 2013). To assess valence, we used a questionnaire developed by Gross and Levenson (1995), which has frequently been applied in emotion research (Goldberg et al. 1999; Han et al. 2012; Lerner et al. 2013). The questionnaire asks subjects to rate their current feelings along 16 different emotional states. We assessed happiness as a composite measure of the items “happy” and “joyful” (Mayer et al. 1992).
To measure arousal, we gathered data on subjects’ skin conductance responses (SCR) (Crone et al. 2004; Døjbak Håkonsson et al. 2016; Van ’t et al. 2006). This is a valid psychophysiological measure, given that emotions are frequently felt outside of conscious awareness (Dawson et al. 2011; Figner and Murphy 2011). To measure arousal, we asked participants to wear wristbands with built-in electrodes during the experiment. The collected data were analyzed using Ledalab, a Matlab-based application. Following the process suggested by Benedek and Kaernbach (2010) we derived the SCR score as the average phasic driver activity after the start of the simulation. To generate this value, we continuously measured participants’ skin conductance response during the experiment. We then used a continuous decomposition analysis (CDA) and a sampling frequency of 4 Hz to decompose SCR data into continuous tonic and phasic driver activity. The phasic driver activity reflects the sudomotor nerve (SN) activity, which serves as an indicator of emotional arousal. Specifically, the CDA provides SCR amplitude values for each participant during the experiment. The average of these SCR amplitude values serves as the participant’s SCR score (Benedek and Kaernbach 2010). For 35 of the 187 subjects, the wristbands did not record any data, as they were moved or accidentally turned off during the experiment. As a result, we obtained valid SCR values from 152 subjects.
Strategic investment decision. In line with Staw (1976) and Tosi et al. (2003), we measured strategic investment decision based on participants’ allocations of the USD 20 million R&D budget during the in-basket exercise. We evaluated the extent to which their strategic investment decisions were diversified or focused along two dimensions. As a first measure, we analyzed the amount invested in R&D for the division with the highest cumulative projected profitability for the years 2018 to 2020 (i.e., the Medical Industrial Products Division; see Appendices A and B). To compare groups, we calculated the mean R&D investment in this division in each group. A lower amount indicates a more diversified strategic investment decision, while a higher amount signals a more focused strategic investment decision.
As a second measure of focused or diversified strategic investment decisions, we followed Li et al. (2018) and assessed the mean distribution of investments across all possible alternatives. In line with Payne (1976), we calculated the standard deviation (SD) of the investment allocation for each individual participant. To compare groups, we calculated the mean standard deviation for each group (i.e., happy and neutral Germans, and happy and neutral Chinese). For example, a value of 6 indicates that the subjects’ investments across alternatives in this particular group differ from the mean by an average of USD 6 million. A high mean SD indicates a high concentration of investments on one alternative—that is, a focused strategic investment decision (i.e., subjects invest very different amounts of money in the three alternatives, thus clearly choosing one), while a low mean SD signals a rather equal distribution of investments across alternatives—that is, a more diversified strategic investment decision (i.e., subjects invest about the same amount of money in each alternative, thus not clearly choosing one).
Control variables for randomization. We included age as a demographic variable for our randomization check, as it has been shown to influence strategic decision making (Wiersema and Bantel 1992) and to affect subjects’ dialecticism (Ng and Hynie 2014). Gender was included, as it has been found to influence emotions (Crawford et al. 1992). In line with Jago and Vroom (1977), we asked participants to indicate their hierarchical level in their organization, as this might influence their familiarity with strategic decisions (Olson et al. 2007, 2016; Pavett and Lau 1983). Work tenure has been shown to influence risk-taking propensity in strategic decisions (Simsek 2007) and is frequently included in studies using in-basket decision-making tasks (Staw and Barsade 1993). We measured work tenure as the number of years that subjects had been working after finishing their academic education. In line with Bantel and Jackson (1989), we included the highest academic degree as a demographic variable in our manipulation checks, as it is frequently used in research on executives (Daellenbach et al. 1999; Datta and Guthrie 1994). Finally, to test for differences in information processing between the Chinese and German participants, we assessed subjects’ holistic tendency based on a 10-item questionnaire developed by Choi et al. (2003) using a 7-point Likert scale. Holistic tendency has been shown to shape cultural differences in information processing (Choi et al. 2003), which made it highly relevant in the context of our study. The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.689.

6 Results

6.1 Randomization Checks

To test whether our experimental groups were successfully randomized, we compared our subgroups with regard to participants’ age, gender, hierarchy, work tenure, and academic degree. Neither the comparison of the German group with the Chinese group nor the comparison of the happy group with the neutral group revealed any differences in these variables, suggesting successful randomization (Tables 1 and 2).
Table 1
Descriptive statistics for German and Chinese executives
Variable
n
Mean
SD
Min
Max
Age
Chinese
97
34.94
9.33
22.00
59.00
German
90
37.14
10.61
25.00
65.00
Gender
Chinese
97
1.35
0.48
1.00
2.00
German
90
1.22
0.42
1.00
2.00
Hierarchy
Chinese
97
2.23
0.91
1.00
4.00
German
90
2.08
1.10
1.00
4.00
Work Tenure
Chinese
97
9.64
8.00
2.00
34.00
German
87a
13.14
12.71
2.00
44.00
Degree
Chinese
97
1.82
0.38
1.00
2.00
German
90
1.74
0.44
1.00
2.00
Degree with 1 = < bachelors degree; 2 = > bachelors degree
a3 missing entries
Table 2
Descriptive statistics for happy and neutral executives
Variable
n
Mean
SD
Min
Max
Age
Happy
93
35.88
9.87
22.00
65.00
Neutral
94
36.12
10.17
23.00
63.00
Gender
Happy
93
1.30
0.46
1.00
2.00
Neutral
94
1.22
0.45
1.00
2.00
Hierarchy
Happy
93
2.15
0.96
1.00
4.00
Neutral
94
2.16
1.01
1.00
4.00
Work Tenure
Happy
91a
10.99
10.44
2.00
44.00
Neutral
93b
11.59
10.80
2.00
43.00
Degree
Happy
93
1.82
0.39
1.00
2.00
Neutral
94
1.76
0.43
1.00
2.00
Degree with 1 = < bachelors degree; 2 = > bachelors degree
a2 missing entries
b1 missing entry
To ensure that our Chinese and German subgroups displayed information-processing differences, we analyzed both groups’ holistic tendencies (Nisbett et al. 2001). An analysis of variance (ANOVA) with culture as the between-subjects factor supports our arguments and is in line with prior research in the field. We found a significant difference between Chinese and German managers (F = 6.386; p = 0.013) with a medium effect size (d = 0.53; r = 0.26) (Cohen 1988), with the Chinese participants (\(\overline{x}\) = 5.59) being more holistic than the German participants (\(\overline{x}\) = 5.15) (See Fig. 1).

6.2 Manipulation Checks

To ensure that we successfully induced happiness in our treatment group, we conducted manipulation checks for our self-reported measure of valence and the SCR measure of arousal. For valence, we found significantly higher values for happiness (German: F = 33.459; p = < 0.0005; Chinese: F = 100.490; p = < 0.0005) in both the German (happy: \(\overline{x}\) = 4.87; neutral: \(\overline{x}\) = 3.03) and the Chinese groups (happy: \(\overline{x}\) = 6.07; neutral: \(\overline{x}\) = 2.36). In line with prior research that has used movie clips to induce emotions (Bramesfeld and Gasper 2008), we observed very large effect sizes in both groups (Germans: d = 1.23; Chinese: d = 2.06) (Cohen 2008).
For the SCR, we found significant differences (F = 6.073; p = 0.015) between the happy (\(\overline{x}\) = 0.15) and the neutral groups (\(\overline{x}\) = 0.10) in our study. The effect size was small (d = 0.40; r = 0.20) (Cohen 1988). Moreover, among the Chinese participants, differences between the happy (\(\overline{x}\) = 0.18) and the neutral (\(\overline{x}\) = 0.12) group were significant (F = 4.027; p = 0.048) with a small effect size (Chinese: d = 0.45; r = 0.22) (Cohen 1988). For the German group, we observed higher values of arousal in the happy group (\(\overline{x}\) = 0.12) than in the neutral group (\(\overline{x}\) = 0.07) but these differences were not statistically significant (F Welch = 3.221; p = 0.077). Overall, however, we conclude that happiness was successfully manipulated in the experiment.

6.3 Hypotheses Tests

To test Hypothesis 1, which suggests that Chinese decision makers diversify their strategic investments more than German executives, we compared both the amount invested in the division with the highest cumulative projected profitability (i.e., the Medical Industrial Products Division) and the standard deviation (SD) of investments across all alternatives. We ran two separate ANOVAs with culture as the between-subjects factor for the 94 participants in our control groups (Lechuga and Wiebe 2011; Li et al. 2016). Tables 3 and 4 show the respective results.
Table 3
ANOVA results for sum of R&D investment for executives in the neutral condition
Group
n
Mean
SD
ANOVA results
p-value
Chinese
49
9.56
4.95
F—Welch
34.801***
< 0.0005
German
45
15.98
5.54
R2
0.276
Adj. R2
0.269
Table 4
ANOVA results for SD of investments for executives in the neutral condition
Group
n
Mean
SD
ANOVA results
p-value
Chinese
49
3.40
2.96
F—Welch
27.445***
< 0.0005
German
45
6.90
3.47
R2
0.232
Adj. R2
0.224
For R&D investments in the division with the highest cumulative projected profitability, we find a significant difference between German and Chinese managers (F Welch = 34.801, p = < 0.0005), with Germans making more investments in the division (\(\overline{x}\) = 15.98) than Chinese participants (\(\overline{x}\) = 9.56). In line with Hypothesis 1, this indicates that Chinese executives seek more diversified strategic investments. The effect size is large (d = 1.23; r = 0.53) (Cohen 1988) (Table 3). For our second measure of strategic investment, the SD of investments across all alternatives, we also find significant differences between the groups (F = 27.445; p = < 0.0005). More specifically, Chinese executives distributed their investments more (\(\overline{x}\) = 3.40) than German executives (\(\overline{x}\) = 6.90). The effect size is again large (d = 1.08; r = 0.48) (Table 4). Thus, both analyses offer support for Hypothesis 1.
Hypotheses 2a predicts that the strategic investment decisions of Chinese executives become more focused when the managers are in a happy emotional state. To test this hypothesis, we ran ANOVAs with emotion as the between-subjects factor for the Chinese subgroups (i.e., neutral versus happy). For R&D investments in the division with the highest cumulative projected profitability as the dependent variable, the results show that the happy group invested significantly more in this alternative (\(\overline{x}\) = 11.79) than the neutral group (\(\overline{x}\) = 9.56) (F = 4.756; p = 0.032). In this case, the effect size is small (d = 0.45; r = 0.22) (Cohen 1988). The results are shown in Table 5. For our second measure of strategic investment, the SD of investments across all alternatives, the results point in the proposed direction, with happy Chinese executives making more focused strategic investments (\(\overline{x}\) = 4.34) than managers in the neutral group (\(\overline{x}\) = 3.40). However, this difference is not significant (F = 2.32; p = 0.131). Thus, our results offer partial support for Hypotheses 2a.
Table 5
ANOVA results for sum of R&D investment of Chinese executives in a happy versus a neutral condition
Group
n
Mean
SD
ANOVA results
p-value
Happy
48
11.79
5.12
F
4.756 *
0.032
Neutral
49
9.56
4.95
R2
0.048
Adj. R2
0.038
Hypothesis 2b predicts that the strategic investment decisions of German executives become more diversified when those executives are in a happy emotional state. To test this hypothesis, we ran ANOVAs with emotion as the between-subjects factor for the German (neutral versus happy) subgroups. For R&D investments in the division with the highest cumulative projected profitability as the dependent variable, the results show that the happy group (\(\overline{x}\) = 13.33) invested significantly less in this alternative than the neutral group (\(\overline{x}\) = 15.98) (F = 4.334; p = 0.040). The effect size is small (d = 0.44; r = 0.22) (Cohen 1988). The results are shown in Table 6. For our second measure of strategic investment, the SD of investments across all alternatives, the results point in the proposed direction, with happy German executives making more diverse strategic investments (\(\overline{x}\) = 5.67) than German managers in the neutral group (\(\overline{x}\) = 6.90). However, the differences is not significant (F = 2.64; p = 0.108). Thus, our results offer partial support for Hypotheses 2b.
Table 6
ANOVA results for sum of R&D investment of German executives in a happy versus a neutral condition
Group
n
Mean
SD
ANOVA results
p-value
Happy
45
13.33
6.47
F
4.334 *
0.04
Neutral
45
15.98
5.54
R2
0.047
Adj. R2
0.036
Hypothesis 2c predicts that happiness moderates the relationship between culture and strategic investments, such that cultural differences between the Chinese and German groups diminish in a happy emotional state. To test this hypothesis, we followed prior research examining the interaction of culture and emotion (Hunsinger et al. 2012; Mok and Morris 2010) and used a 2 × 2 factorial design with culture (German versus Chinese) and emotion (happy versus neutral) as factors, and both R&D investments in the Medical Industrial Products Division and the SD of investment as dependent variables. For R&D investments in the Medical Industrial Products Division, the analysis reveals a significant main effect of culture (F = 24.136; p < 0.0005) with a medium effect size (d = 0.72; r = 0.34) (Cohen 1988), but no main effect of emotion (F = 0.065; p = 0.799). At the same time, the analysis points to a significant interaction effect of culture and emotion (F = 9.056, p = 0.003) (Table 7). We obtain similar results for the SD of investments, our second measure of strategic investment. Culture has a highly significant main effect (F = 24.730; p < 0.0005) with a medium effect size (d = 0.73; r = 0.35) (Cohen 1988), while no significant main effect of emotion occurs (F = 0.088; p = 0.767). Again, the analysis shows a significant interaction effect of culture and emotion (F = 5.002; p = 0.027) (Table 8). These results offer support for Hypothesis 2c.
Table 7
ANOVA results for the interaction between culture (German versus Chinese managers) and emotion (neutral versus happy emotional state) on the sum of R&D investments
 
n
ANOVA results
p-value
Emotion
187
F
0.065 n. s.
0.799
Culture
187
F
24.136***
< 0.0005
Culture × Emotion
187
F
9.056**
0.003
R2
0.154
Adj. R2
0.140
Table 8
ANOVA results for the interaction between culture (German vs. Chinese managers) and emotion (neutral vs. happy emotional state) on SD of investment
Variable
n
ANOVA results
p-value
Emotion
187
F
0.088 n. s.
0.767
Culture
187
F
24.73***
< 0.0005
Culture × Emotion
187
F
5.002*
0.027
R2
0.140
Adj. R2
0.126
We also plotted the interactions for both the invested amount and the SD of investments. These plots are depicted in Figs. 2 and 3.
To further support the robustness of our findings, we used ANOVAs to analyze whether cultural differences disappear between the subgroups of happy Germans and happy Chinese. For R&D investments in the division with the highest projected absolute cumulative profitability, we find no significant difference between the German (\(\overline{x}\) = 13.33; SD = 6.47) and Chinese subgroups (\(\overline{x}\) = 11.79; SD = 5.12) (F Welch = 1.609; p = 0.208). Similarly, when analyzing the mean SD of investments, no significant difference (F = 3.470; p = 0.066) emerges between the German (\(\overline{x}\) = 5.67; SD = 3.70) and Chinese subgroups (\(\overline{x}\) = 4.35; SD = 3.13). These results, which are shown in Tables 9 and 10, offer further support for Hypothesis 2c.
Table 9
ANOVA results for sum of R&D investment for all executives in the happy condition
Group
n
Mean
SD
ANOVA results
p-value
Chinese
48
11.79
5.12
F—Welch
1.609 n. s.
0.208
German
45
13.33
6.47
R2
0.018
Adj. R2
0.007
Table 10
ANOVA results for SD of investment for all executives in the happy condition
Group
n
Mean
SD
ANOVA results
p-value
Chinese
48
4.35
3.13
F—Welch
3.47 n. s.
0.066
German
45
5.67
3.7
R2
0.037
Adj. R2
0.027

7 Discussion

Cultural differences exert important effects on strategic decision making (Dimitratos et al. 2011) and organizational performance (Li et al. 2001). While the extant research on the impact of culture in strategic decision making is mostly grounded in the values-based approach to culture (Hofstede 1980) and regards cultural dispositions as static, psychological research has applied a dynamic constructivist approach. This approach suggests that the cultural tendencies of decision makers may be altered by contingency factors in the decision process. We argue that emotions constitute an important boundary condition, as they have been shown to alter cultural predispositions in both Western (Bramesfeld and Gasper 2008; Staw and Barsade 1993) and Asian contexts (Au et al. 2003). Empirically, we find that although German and Chinese executives differ significantly in their strategic investment behavior because of their cultural dispositions, happiness serves as a boundary condition that reverses cultural dispositions (Choi et al. 2003).
As such, our findings make two theoretical contributions to the strategic decision-making literature in particular and strategic management research in general. First, we contribute to strategic decision-making research by introducing the dynamic constructivist approach to culture and by identifying emotions, specifically happiness, as a boundary condition for the effects of cultural differences on strategic decision making. Thus, we provide support for the notion that culture is a dynamic, rather than a static, driver of strategic decision making (Ronen and Shenkar 2013). Future research should focus on asking when and not just how cultural differences matter. In addition, our study provides further evidence of the suggested interdependence between emotion and cognition in an intercultural context (Forgas and George 2001; Kim 2012). By examining the simultaneous effects of culture and emotion, we are able to provide an explanation for Au et al. (2003) findings, which contradict those of research conducted with Western subjects (Bramesfeld and Gasper 2008; Estrada et al. 1997; Isen et al. 1991; Staw and Barsade 1993). Additionally, we add to findings by Neumann and Wulf (2022) and show that emotions serve as a boundary condition for the effect of cultural differences beyond the mere interpretation of strategic issues. Rather, our study highlights that emotion might take on this role throughout the strategic decision-making process. Overall, our findings show that a more differentiated understanding of cognitive processes in the strategic decision-making process can be gained by examining contingency factors, such as emotions. This might also contribute to a better understanding of other cognitive processes involved in decision making (e.g., attribution, creativity, or memory) (Lerner et al. 2015).
The second contribution of our research goes beyond research on strategic decision making. By identifying happiness as a moderator of cultural dispositions, we support other researchers’ calls for more research on emotional and cognitive processes in strategy management research (Burgelman et al. 2018). By integrating social and cognitive psychology research into strategic management research, we demonstrate the additional explanatory power offered by research within the field of behavioral strategy (Huang 2009; Pettigrew et al. 2001). In particular, our findings highlight the need for a shift in the discussion from examining which cultural differences exist towards examining the conditions under which they change. This shift will extend the focus of strategic management research from regarding culture as a fixed cross-cultural construct and might inform research on, for instance, the influence of cultural diversity on strategic decision outcomes (Nielsen and Nielsen 2013) or research on the consequences of cultural distance between headquarters and foreign subsidiaries (Plourde et al. 2014). It may also enhance research on the role of cultural differences in other strategic processes, such as opportunity identification (Shepherd et al. 2017).
Beyond these theoretical contributions, our study makes several empirical contributions to strategic decision-making research. Building on research in the fields of cognitive and social psychology, our study offers one of the first examinations of the effect of emotion on strategic decision-making behavior in a previously largely ignored cultural group: the Chinese (Ashkanasy and Dorris 2017). Furthermore, our study is the first to consider the simultaneous effects of emotion and culture on decision making. Finally, by conducting an in-basket strategic decision-making experiment, our study extends research on emotion in the strategic management literature by examining actual individual decision-making behavior rather than decision-making outcomes (Choi et al. 2011; Døjbak Håkonsson et al. 2016; Huy 2011) or conceptual contributions in the field (Hodgkinson and Healey 2011; Sanchez-Burks and Huy 2009).
Finally, our study has some important practical implications. Specifically, organizations and top managers should be careful to take the effects of emotions, such as happiness, into account when making strategic decisions. While we make no inferences about the impact of emotions on the quality of strategic decision making, organizations should make their decision-makers aware of both cultural and emotional influences during decision making. As emotions represent an integral part of daily work (Ashkanasy and Dorris 2017), learning how to handle them and becoming aware of their effects, especially in a multinational context, might be important for both the quality and process of decision making as well as subsequent organizational performance (Seo and Barrett 2007). Furthermore, in managing intercultural interactions (e.g., between headquarters and subsidiaries, or among members of internationally diversified top management teams), an awareness of the moderating influence of emotion on cultural differences might even reduce conflict and discussions around decision making. Thus, being happy together might also make us better able to work together.

8 Limitations and Future Research

All research has its limitations. As such, this study suffers from several limitations that offer opportunities for future research. First, we only examined the influences of emotion on Germans and Chinese as representatives of individualistic and collectivistic cultures, respectively. Thus, future research might enhance our findings by examining their generalizability to other cultural groups. Such research could, for example, examine Japanese and Americans in order to include two more frequently examined cultural groups (Yates and de Oliveira 2016).
Second, as we used Staw’s (1976) Adam & Smith case, we exposed subjects to a specific strategic situation. Although in-basket exercises provide a high degree of realism and have been shown to be good predictors of managerial behavior (Woodside et al. 2016), whether the moderating effect of happiness on cross-cultural differences can be extended to other strategic decisions (e.g., market-entry situations) is a question for future research. For instance, researchers could use business simulations or vignette decision experiments to increase the generalizability of our findings beyond the specific strategy context and the methodology used here.
Third, in our study we followed the dynamic constructivist approach to culture (Hong and Chiu 2001) which regards the social norms, values, and cognitive styles, that are shared among the members of a cultural group, as drivers of differences in their decision-making behavior (e.g., Yates and de Oliveira 2016). While this approach is well-established in psychological research, it neither accounts for within-culture variation, which might result from differences in educational systems, politics, or religious beliefs within East Asian or Western cultures, nor for variations that are caused by an interplay of national and organizational cultures (i.e., an East Asian individual working for a Western company). Thus, future research might benefit from a more fine-grained analysis of the drivers of social norms, values, and cognitive styles that are characteristic of a cultural group as well as from testing the effects of organizational compared to national cultures on strategic decision making.
Finally, our research only addressed the effect of one specific emotion on cultural differences in strategic decision making—namely of happiness. While happiness is an important emotion in the context of strategic decision making (Hermalin and Isen 2000), future research should also examine the contingency effects of other distinct emotional states. A great deal of research has, for example, been conducted on the influence of anger and fear (Lerner and Tiedens 2006; Tiedens and Linton 2001). Additionally, such research might look beyond the effects of individual decision making to analyze how emotions alter cultural dispositions in strategy discussions and the subsequent decision outcomes of a group (Liu and Maitlis 2014).

9 Conclusion

Culture plays an important role in strategic management research given cross-cultural differences in strategic orientation and decision making. Based on the dynamic constructivist approach to culture, this study examines not only how but also when these differences matter. Our results suggest that emotion, especially happiness, is a significant moderator of cultural tendencies in strategic decision-making behavior. Specifically, we find that while the Germans and the Chinese differ significantly in their decision-making behavior in a neutral state, these differences vanish in a happy state. Overall, the findings of this paper significantly contribute to research on strategic decision making in particular and strategic management in general, and have important practical implications for companies operating in an international environment.

Conflict of interest

S. Florian, P. Meissner, T. Wulf, X. Xu and P. Yang declare that they have no competing interests.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge

Appendix A

Exemplary In-basket Items

Appendix B

Decision-making Task

Please use the boxes below to allocate the indicated R&D budget of USD 20 million among the divisions. As the memorandum from the board meeting indicated, your decision should be based on the projected cumulative absolute profitability per division that can be reached with the investment until 2020. The entire amount may be allocated to one division, or split among two or all divisions. Please enter the amount in millions.
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Amabile, T.M., S.G. Barsade, J.S. Mueller, and B.M. Staw. 2005. Affect and creativity at work. Administrative science quarterly 50(3):367–403.CrossRef Amabile, T.M., S.G. Barsade, J.S. Mueller, and B.M. Staw. 2005. Affect and creativity at work. Administrative science quarterly 50(3):367–403.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ashkanasy, N.M., and A.D. Dorris. 2017. Emotions in the workplace. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior 4(1):67–90.CrossRef Ashkanasy, N.M., and A.D. Dorris. 2017. Emotions in the workplace. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior 4(1):67–90.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Au, K., F. Chan, D. Wang, and I. Vertinsky. 2003. Mood in foreign exchange trading: cognitive processes and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 91(2):322–338.CrossRef Au, K., F. Chan, D. Wang, and I. Vertinsky. 2003. Mood in foreign exchange trading: cognitive processes and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 91(2):322–338.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bachmann, J.T., A. Engelen, and C. Schwens. 2016. Toward a better understanding of the association between strategic planning and entrepreneurial orientation—The moderating role of national culture. Journal of International Management 22(4):297–315.CrossRef Bachmann, J.T., A. Engelen, and C. Schwens. 2016. Toward a better understanding of the association between strategic planning and entrepreneurial orientation—The moderating role of national culture. Journal of International Management 22(4):297–315.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bantel, K.A., and S.E. Jackson. 1989. Top management and innovations in banking: does the composition of the top team make a difference? Strategic Management Journal 10(S1):107–124.CrossRef Bantel, K.A., and S.E. Jackson. 1989. Top management and innovations in banking: does the composition of the top team make a difference? Strategic Management Journal 10(S1):107–124.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Barr, P.S., and M.A. Glynn. 2004. Cultural variations in strategic issue interpretation: relating cultural uncertainty avoidance to controllability in discriminating threat and opportunity. Strategic Management Journal 25(1):59–67.CrossRef Barr, P.S., and M.A. Glynn. 2004. Cultural variations in strategic issue interpretation: relating cultural uncertainty avoidance to controllability in discriminating threat and opportunity. Strategic Management Journal 25(1):59–67.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Benedek, M., and C. Kaernbach. 2010. A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 190(1):80–91.CrossRef Benedek, M., and C. Kaernbach. 2010. A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 190(1):80–91.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Beugelsdijk, S., R. Maseland, and A. Van Hoorn. 2015. Are scores on Hofstede’s dimensions of national culture stable over time? A cohort analysis. Global Strategy Journal 5(3):223–240.CrossRef Beugelsdijk, S., R. Maseland, and A. Van Hoorn. 2015. Are scores on Hofstede’s dimensions of national culture stable over time? A cohort analysis. Global Strategy Journal 5(3):223–240.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bramesfeld, K.D., and K. Gasper. 2008. Happily putting the pieces together: a test of two explanations for the effects of mood on group-level information processing. British Journal of Social Psychology 47(2):285–309.CrossRef Bramesfeld, K.D., and K. Gasper. 2008. Happily putting the pieces together: a test of two explanations for the effects of mood on group-level information processing. British Journal of Social Psychology 47(2):285–309.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Briley, D., R.S. Wyer Jr, and E. Li. 2014. A dynamic view of cultural influence: a review. Journal of Consumer Psychology 24(4):557–571.CrossRef Briley, D., R.S. Wyer Jr, and E. Li. 2014. A dynamic view of cultural influence: a review. Journal of Consumer Psychology 24(4):557–571.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Briley, D.A., M.W. Morris, and I. Simonson. 2000. Reasons as carriers of culture: Dynamic versus dispositional models of cultural influence on decision making. Journal of Consumer Research 27(2):157-178.CrossRef Briley, D.A., M.W. Morris, and I. Simonson. 2000. Reasons as carriers of culture: Dynamic versus dispositional models of cultural influence on decision making. Journal of Consumer Research 27(2):157-178.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brislin, R.W. 1970. Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 1(3):185–216.CrossRef Brislin, R.W. 1970. Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 1(3):185–216.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chiu, C.Y., M.W. Morris, Y.Y. Hong, and T. Menon. 2000. Motivated cultural cognition: the impact of implicit cultural theories on dispositional attribution varies as a function of need for closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78(2):247–259.CrossRef Chiu, C.Y., M.W. Morris, Y.Y. Hong, and T. Menon. 2000. Motivated cultural cognition: the impact of implicit cultural theories on dispositional attribution varies as a function of need for closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78(2):247–259.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Choi, B.J. 2020. The dynamic influence of culture on variation in consumers’ attachment of value to a product. Journal of Consumer Marketing 37(5):533–545.CrossRef Choi, B.J. 2020. The dynamic influence of culture on variation in consumers’ attachment of value to a product. Journal of Consumer Marketing 37(5):533–545.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Choi, I., R. Dalal, C. Kim-Prieto, and H. Park. 2003. Culture and judgment of causal relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84(1):46–59.CrossRef Choi, I., R. Dalal, C. Kim-Prieto, and H. Park. 2003. Culture and judgment of causal relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84(1):46–59.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Choi, J.N., S.Y. Sung, K. Lee, and D.-S. Cho. 2011. Balancing cognition and emotion: innovation implementation as a function of cognitive appraisal and emotional reactions toward innovation. Journal of Organizational Behavior 32(1):107–124.CrossRef Choi, J.N., S.Y. Sung, K. Lee, and D.-S. Cho. 2011. Balancing cognition and emotion: innovation implementation as a function of cognitive appraisal and emotional reactions toward innovation. Journal of Organizational Behavior 32(1):107–124.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen, B.H. 2008. Explaining psychological statistics, 3rd edn., Hoboken: Wiley. Cohen, B.H. 2008. Explaining psychological statistics, 3rd edn., Hoboken: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen, J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn., Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cohen, J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn., Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Connelly, S., W. Helton-Fauth, and M.D. Mumford. 2004. A managerial in-basket study of the impact of trait emotions on ethical choice. Journal of Business Ethics 51(3):245–267.CrossRef Connelly, S., W. Helton-Fauth, and M.D. Mumford. 2004. A managerial in-basket study of the impact of trait emotions on ethical choice. Journal of Business Ethics 51(3):245–267.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Crawford, J., S. Kippax, J. Onyx, U. Gault, and P. Benton. 1992. Emotion and gender: constructing meaning from memory. SAGE. Crawford, J., S. Kippax, J. Onyx, U. Gault, and P. Benton. 1992. Emotion and gender: constructing meaning from memory. SAGE.
Zurück zum Zitat Crone, E.A., R.J.M. Somsen, B. van Beek, and M.W. van der Molen. 2004. Heart rate and skin conductance analysis of antecedents and consequences of decision making. Psychophysiology 41(4):531–540.CrossRef Crone, E.A., R.J.M. Somsen, B. van Beek, and M.W. van der Molen. 2004. Heart rate and skin conductance analysis of antecedents and consequences of decision making. Psychophysiology 41(4):531–540.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Croson, R., J. Anand, and R. Agarwal. 2007. Using experiments in corporate strategy research. European Management Review 4(3):173–181.CrossRef Croson, R., J. Anand, and R. Agarwal. 2007. Using experiments in corporate strategy research. European Management Review 4(3):173–181.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Daellenbach, U.S., A.M. McCarthy, and T.S. Schoenecker. 1999. Commitment to innovation: The impact of top management team characteristics. R&D Management 29(3):199–208.CrossRef Daellenbach, U.S., A.M. McCarthy, and T.S. Schoenecker. 1999. Commitment to innovation: The impact of top management team characteristics. R&D Management 29(3):199–208.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Datta, D.K., and J.P. Guthrie. 1994. Executive succession: organizational antecedents of CEO characteristics. Strategic Management Journal 15(7):569–577.CrossRef Datta, D.K., and J.P. Guthrie. 1994. Executive succession: organizational antecedents of CEO characteristics. Strategic Management Journal 15(7):569–577.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dawson, M.E., A.M. Schell, and C.G. Courtney. 2011. The skin conductance response, anticipation, and decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics 4(2):111–116.CrossRef Dawson, M.E., A.M. Schell, and C.G. Courtney. 2011. The skin conductance response, anticipation, and decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics 4(2):111–116.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dimitratos, P., A. Petrou, E. Plakoyiannaki, and J.E. Johnson. 2011. Strategic decision-making processes in internationalization: Does national culture of the focal firm matter? Journal of World Business 46(2):194–204.CrossRef Dimitratos, P., A. Petrou, E. Plakoyiannaki, and J.E. Johnson. 2011. Strategic decision-making processes in internationalization: Does national culture of the focal firm matter? Journal of World Business 46(2):194–204.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Døjbak Håkonsson, D., J.K. Eskildsen, L. Argote, D. Mønster, R.M. Burton, and B. Obel. 2016. Exploration versus exploitation: emotions and performance as antecedents and consequences of team decisions. Strategic Management Journal 37(6):985–1001.CrossRef Døjbak Håkonsson, D., J.K. Eskildsen, L. Argote, D. Mønster, R.M. Burton, and B. Obel. 2016. Exploration versus exploitation: emotions and performance as antecedents and consequences of team decisions. Strategic Management Journal 37(6):985–1001.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Elfenbein, H.A. 2007. Emotion in organizations: a review and theoretical integration. The Academy of Management Annals 1(1):315–386.CrossRef Elfenbein, H.A. 2007. Emotion in organizations: a review and theoretical integration. The Academy of Management Annals 1(1):315–386.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Estrada, C.A., A.M. Isen, and M.J. Young. 1994. Positive affect improves creative problem solving and influences reported source of practice satisfaction in physicians. Motivation and Emotion 18(4):285–299.CrossRef Estrada, C.A., A.M. Isen, and M.J. Young. 1994. Positive affect improves creative problem solving and influences reported source of practice satisfaction in physicians. Motivation and Emotion 18(4):285–299.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Estrada, C.A., A.M. Isen, and M.J. Young. 1997. Positive affect facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring in reasoning among physicians. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 72(1):117–135.CrossRef Estrada, C.A., A.M. Isen, and M.J. Young. 1997. Positive affect facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring in reasoning among physicians. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 72(1):117–135.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Figner, B., and R.O. Murphy. 2011. Using skin conductance in judgment and decision making research. In Society for judgment and decision making series. A handbook of process tracing methods for decision research: a critical review and user’s guide, ed. M. Schulte-Mecklenbeck, 163–184. New York: Psychology Press. Figner, B., and R.O. Murphy. 2011. Using skin conductance in judgment and decision making research. In Society for judgment and decision making series. A handbook of process tracing methods for decision research: a critical review and user’s guide, ed. M. Schulte-Mecklenbeck, 163–184. New York: Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Forgas, J.P. 1998. On being happy and mistaken: mood effects on the fundamental attribution error. Journal of personality and social psychology 75(2):318–331.CrossRef Forgas, J.P. 1998. On being happy and mistaken: mood effects on the fundamental attribution error. Journal of personality and social psychology 75(2):318–331.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Forgas, J.P., and J.M. George. 2001. Affective influences on judgments and behavior in organizations: an information processing perspective. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 86(1):3–34.CrossRef Forgas, J.P., and J.M. George. 2001. Affective influences on judgments and behavior in organizations: an information processing perspective. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 86(1):3–34.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fredrickson, B.L., and C. Branigan. 2005. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion 19(3):313–332.CrossRef Fredrickson, B.L., and C. Branigan. 2005. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion 19(3):313–332.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Geletkanycz, M.A. 1997. The salience of ‘culture’s consequences’: the effects of cultural values on top executive commitment to the status quo. Strategic Management Journal 18(8):615–634.CrossRef Geletkanycz, M.A. 1997. The salience of ‘culture’s consequences’: the effects of cultural values on top executive commitment to the status quo. Strategic Management Journal 18(8):615–634.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat George, J.M., and E. Dane. 2016. Affect, emotion, and decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136:47–55.CrossRef George, J.M., and E. Dane. 2016. Affect, emotion, and decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136:47–55.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goldberg, J.H., J.S. Lerner, and P.E. Tetlock. 1999. Rage and reason: the psychology of the intuitive prosecutor. European Journal of Social Psychology 29.. Goldberg, J.H., J.S. Lerner, and P.E. Tetlock. 1999. Rage and reason: the psychology of the intuitive prosecutor. European Journal of Social Psychology 29..
Zurück zum Zitat Gomes, C.F., C.J. Brainerd, and L.M. Stein. 2013. Effects of emotional valence and arousal on recollective and nonrecollective recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 39(3):663–677. Gomes, C.F., C.J. Brainerd, and L.M. Stein. 2013. Effects of emotional valence and arousal on recollective and nonrecollective recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 39(3):663–677.
Zurück zum Zitat Grimm, P. 2010. Social desirability bias. In Wiley international encyclopedia of marketing, Vol. 6, ed. J.N. Sheth, N.K. Malhotra. Hoboken: Wiley. Grimm, P. 2010. Social desirability bias. In Wiley international encyclopedia of marketing, Vol. 6, ed. J.N. Sheth, N.K. Malhotra. Hoboken: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Gross, J.J., and R.W. Levenson. 1995. Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition & Emotion 9(1):87–108.CrossRef Gross, J.J., and R.W. Levenson. 1995. Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition & Emotion 9(1):87–108.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Han, S., J.S. Lerner, and D. Keltner. 2007. Feelings and consumer decision-making: the appraisal tendency framework. Journal of Consumer Psychology 17(3):158–168.CrossRef Han, S., J.S. Lerner, and D. Keltner. 2007. Feelings and consumer decision-making: the appraisal tendency framework. Journal of Consumer Psychology 17(3):158–168.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Han, S., J.S. Lerner, and R. Zeckhauser. 2012. The disgust-promotes-disposal effect. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 44(2):101–113.CrossRef Han, S., J.S. Lerner, and R. Zeckhauser. 2012. The disgust-promotes-disposal effect. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 44(2):101–113.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hermalin, B.E., and A.M. Isen. 2000. The effect of affect on economic and strategic decision making. USC CLEO Research Paper, (C01–5).CrossRef Hermalin, B.E., and A.M. Isen. 2000. The effect of affect on economic and strategic decision making. USC CLEO Research Paper, (C01–5).CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hodgkinson, G.P., and M.P. Healey. 2008. Cognition in organizations. Annual Review of Psychology 59:387–417.CrossRef Hodgkinson, G.P., and M.P. Healey. 2008. Cognition in organizations. Annual Review of Psychology 59:387–417.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hodgkinson, G.P., and M.P. Healey. 2011. Psychological foundations of dynamic capabilities: reflexion and reflection in strategic management. Strategic Management Journal 32(13):1500–1516.CrossRef Hodgkinson, G.P., and M.P. Healey. 2011. Psychological foundations of dynamic capabilities: reflexion and reflection in strategic management. Strategic Management Journal 32(13):1500–1516.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hofstede, G.H. 1980. Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. London: SAGE. Hofstede, G.H. 1980. Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. London: SAGE.
Zurück zum Zitat Hong, Y. 2009. A dynamic constructivist approach to culture: moving from describing culture to explaining culture. In Understanding culture: theory, research and application, ed. R.S. Wyer, C. Chiu, and Y. Hong, 3–23. New York: Psychology Press. Hong, Y. 2009. A dynamic constructivist approach to culture: moving from describing culture to explaining culture. In Understanding culture: theory, research and application, ed. R.S. Wyer, C. Chiu, and Y. Hong, 3–23. New York: Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Hong, Y.Y., and C.Y. Chiu. 2001. Toward a paradigm shift: From cross-cultural differences in social cognition to social-cognitive mediation of cultural differences. Social Cognition 19(3):181–196.CrossRef Hong, Y.Y., and C.Y. Chiu. 2001. Toward a paradigm shift: From cross-cultural differences in social cognition to social-cognitive mediation of cultural differences. Social Cognition 19(3):181–196.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hong, Y.Y., M.W. Morris, C.Y. Chiu, and V. Benet-Martinez. 2000. Multicultural minds: a dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American psychologist 55(7):709.CrossRef Hong, Y.Y., M.W. Morris, C.Y. Chiu, and V. Benet-Martinez. 2000. Multicultural minds: a dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American psychologist 55(7):709.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huang, X. 2009. Strategic decision making in Chinese SMEs. Chinese Management Studies 3(2):87–101.CrossRef Huang, X. 2009. Strategic decision making in Chinese SMEs. Chinese Management Studies 3(2):87–101.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hunsinger, M., L.M. Isbell, and G.L. Clore. 2012. Sometimes happy people focus on the trees and sad people focus on the forest: context-dependent effects of mood in impression formation. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin 38(2):220–232.CrossRef Hunsinger, M., L.M. Isbell, and G.L. Clore. 2012. Sometimes happy people focus on the trees and sad people focus on the forest: context-dependent effects of mood in impression formation. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin 38(2):220–232.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Huy, Q.N. 2011. How middle managers’ group-focus emotions and social identities influence strategy implementation. Strategic Management Journal 32(13):1387–1410.CrossRef Huy, Q.N. 2011. How middle managers’ group-focus emotions and social identities influence strategy implementation. Strategic Management Journal 32(13):1387–1410.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Isen, A.M., and R.A. Baron. 1991. Positive affect as a factor in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior. Greenwich: JAi Press. Isen, A.M., and R.A. Baron. 1991. Positive affect as a factor in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior. Greenwich: JAi Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Isen, A.M., A.S. Rosenzweig, and M.J. Young. 1991. The influence of positive affect on clinical problem solving. Medical Decision Making 11(3):221–227.CrossRef Isen, A.M., A.S. Rosenzweig, and M.J. Young. 1991. The influence of positive affect on clinical problem solving. Medical Decision Making 11(3):221–227.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jago, A.G., and V.H. Vroom. 1977. Hierarchical level and leadership style. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 18(1):131–145.CrossRef Jago, A.G., and V.H. Vroom. 1977. Hierarchical level and leadership style. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 18(1):131–145.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ji, L.-J. 2005. Culture and lay theories of change. In Culture and social behavior: the ontario symposium, ed. R.M. Sorrentino, D. Cohen, J.M. Olson, and M.P. Zanna, 117–136. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. Ji, L.-J. 2005. Culture and lay theories of change. In Culture and social behavior: the ontario symposium, ed. R.M. Sorrentino, D. Cohen, J.M. Olson, and M.P. Zanna, 117–136. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Ji, L.-J., Z. Zhang, and T. Guo. 2008. To buy or to sell: cultural differences in stock market decisions based on price trends. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 21(4):399–413.CrossRef Ji, L.-J., Z. Zhang, and T. Guo. 2008. To buy or to sell: cultural differences in stock market decisions based on price trends. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 21(4):399–413.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kesselman, G.A., F.M. Lopez, and F.E. Lopez. 1982. The development and validation of a self-report scored in-basket test in an assessment center setting. Personnel Administration 11(3):228–238. Kesselman, G.A., F.M. Lopez, and F.E. Lopez. 1982. The development and validation of a self-report scored in-basket test in an assessment center setting. Personnel Administration 11(3):228–238.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim, K.-H. 2012. Emotion and strategic decision-making behavior: developing a theoretical model. International Journal of Business and Social Science 3(1):105–113. Kim, K.-H. 2012. Emotion and strategic decision-making behavior: developing a theoretical model. International Journal of Business and Social Science 3(1):105–113.
Zurück zum Zitat Kitayama, S., and H.R. Marcus. 1999. Yin and yang of the Japanese self: the cultural psychology of personality coherence. In The coherence of personality: social-cognitive bases of consistency, variability, and organization, ed. D. Cervone, Y. Shoda, 242–302. Guilford. Kitayama, S., and H.R. Marcus. 1999. Yin and yang of the Japanese self: the cultural psychology of personality coherence. In The coherence of personality: social-cognitive bases of consistency, variability, and organization, ed. D. Cervone, Y. Shoda, 242–302. Guilford.
Zurück zum Zitat Kitayama, S., B. Mesquita, and M. Karasawa. 2006. Cultural affordances and emotional experience: socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of personality and social psychology 91(5):890–903.CrossRef Kitayama, S., B. Mesquita, and M. Karasawa. 2006. Cultural affordances and emotional experience: socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of personality and social psychology 91(5):890–903.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kitayama, S., S. Duffy, and Y. Uchida. 2007. Self as cultural mode of being. In Handbook of cultural psychology, 136–174. Kitayama, S., S. Duffy, and Y. Uchida. 2007. Self as cultural mode of being. In Handbook of cultural psychology, 136–174.
Zurück zum Zitat Koudstaal, M., R. Sloof, and M. van Praag. 2016. Risk, uncertainty, and entrepreneurship: evidence from a lab-in-the-field experiment. Management Science 62(10):2897–2915.CrossRef Koudstaal, M., R. Sloof, and M. van Praag. 2016. Risk, uncertainty, and entrepreneurship: evidence from a lab-in-the-field experiment. Management Science 62(10):2897–2915.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kühnen, U., and D. Oyserman. 2002. Thinking about the self influences thinking in general: cognitive consequences of salient self-concept. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 38:492–499.CrossRef Kühnen, U., and D. Oyserman. 2002. Thinking about the self influences thinking in general: cognitive consequences of salient self-concept. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 38:492–499.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lazar, J.N., and S. Pearlman-Avnion. 2014. Effect of affect induction method on emotional valence and arousal. Psychology 05(07):595–601.CrossRef Lazar, J.N., and S. Pearlman-Avnion. 2014. Effect of affect induction method on emotional valence and arousal. Psychology 05(07):595–601.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lechuga, J., and J.S. Wiebe. 2011. Culture and probability judgment accuracy: the influence of holistic reasoning. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(6):1054–1065.CrossRef Lechuga, J., and J.S. Wiebe. 2011. Culture and probability judgment accuracy: the influence of holistic reasoning. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(6):1054–1065.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lerner, J.S., and D. Keltner. 2001. Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81(1):146–159.CrossRef Lerner, J.S., and D. Keltner. 2001. Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81(1):146–159.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lerner, J.S., and L.Z. Tiedens. 2006. Portrait of the angry decision maker: how appraisal tendencies shape anger’s influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 19(2):115–137.CrossRef Lerner, J.S., and L.Z. Tiedens. 2006. Portrait of the angry decision maker: how appraisal tendencies shape anger’s influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 19(2):115–137.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lerner, J.S., Y. Li, and E.U. Weber. 2013. The financial costs of sadness. Psychological Science 24(1):72–79.CrossRef Lerner, J.S., Y. Li, and E.U. Weber. 2013. The financial costs of sadness. Psychological Science 24(1):72–79.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lerner, J.S., Y. Li, P. Valdesolo, and K.S. Kassam. 2015. Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology 66:799–823.CrossRef Lerner, J.S., Y. Li, P. Valdesolo, and K.S. Kassam. 2015. Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology 66:799–823.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Li, C., F.C. Brodbeck, O. Shenkar, L.J. Ponzi, and J.H. Fisch. 2017. Embracing the foreign: cultural attractiveness and international strategy. Strategic Management Journal 38(4):950–971.CrossRef Li, C., F.C. Brodbeck, O. Shenkar, L.J. Ponzi, and J.H. Fisch. 2017. Embracing the foreign: cultural attractiveness and international strategy. Strategic Management Journal 38(4):950–971.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Li, J., K. Lam, and G. Qian. 2001. Does culture affect behavior and performance of firms? The case of joint ventures in China. Journal of International Business Studies 32(1):115–131.CrossRef Li, J., K. Lam, and G. Qian. 2001. Does culture affect behavior and performance of firms? The case of joint ventures in China. Journal of International Business Studies 32(1):115–131.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Li, L.M.W., T. Masuda, and M.J. Russell. 2014. The influence of cultural lay beliefs: dialecticism and indecisiveness in European canadians and Hong Kong Chinese. Personality and Individual Differences 68:6–12.CrossRef Li, L.M.W., T. Masuda, and M.J. Russell. 2014. The influence of cultural lay beliefs: dialecticism and indecisiveness in European canadians and Hong Kong Chinese. Personality and Individual Differences 68:6–12.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Li, L.M.W., T. Masuda, and M.J. Russell. 2015. Culture and decision-making: investigating cultural variations in the East Asian and North American online decision-making processes. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 18(3):183–191.CrossRef Li, L.M.W., T. Masuda, and M.J. Russell. 2015. Culture and decision-making: investigating cultural variations in the East Asian and North American online decision-making processes. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 18(3):183–191.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Li, L.M.W., T. Masuda, and F. Jiang. 2016. Influence of cultural meaning system and socioeconomic change on indecisiveness in three cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 47(4):508–524.CrossRef Li, L.M.W., T. Masuda, and F. Jiang. 2016. Influence of cultural meaning system and socioeconomic change on indecisiveness in three cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 47(4):508–524.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Li, L.M.W., T. Masuda, T. Hamamura, and K. Ishii. 2018. Culture and decision making: influence of analytic versus holistic thinking style on resource allocation in a fort game. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 49(7):1066–1080.CrossRef Li, L.M.W., T. Masuda, T. Hamamura, and K. Ishii. 2018. Culture and decision making: influence of analytic versus holistic thinking style on resource allocation in a fort game. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 49(7):1066–1080.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lindquist, K.A., L.F. Barrett, E. Bliss-Moreau, and J.A. Russell. 2006. Language and the perception of emotion. Emotion 6(1):125–138.CrossRef Lindquist, K.A., L.F. Barrett, E. Bliss-Moreau, and J.A. Russell. 2006. Language and the perception of emotion. Emotion 6(1):125–138.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Liu, F., and S. Maitlis. 2014. Emotional dynamics and strategizing processes: a study of strategic conversations in top team meetings. Journal of Management Studies 51(2):202–234.CrossRef Liu, F., and S. Maitlis. 2014. Emotional dynamics and strategizing processes: a study of strategic conversations in top team meetings. Journal of Management Studies 51(2):202–234.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat López-Duarte, C., M.M. Vidal-Suárez, and B. González-Díaz. 2016. International business and national culture: a literature review and research agenda. International Journal of Management Reviews 18(4):397–416.CrossRef López-Duarte, C., M.M. Vidal-Suárez, and B. González-Díaz. 2016. International business and national culture: a literature review and research agenda. International Journal of Management Reviews 18(4):397–416.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Maitlis, S., and H. Ozcelik. 2004. Toxic decision processes: a study of emotion and organizational decision making. Organization Science 15(4):375–393.CrossRef Maitlis, S., and H. Ozcelik. 2004. Toxic decision processes: a study of emotion and organizational decision making. Organization Science 15(4):375–393.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Matsumoto, D., and J. Wilson. 2008. Culture, emotion and motivation. In Handbook of motivation and cognition across cultures, ed. R.M. Sorrentino, S. Yamaguchi, 541–563. San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRef Matsumoto, D., and J. Wilson. 2008. Culture, emotion and motivation. In Handbook of motivation and cognition across cultures, ed. R.M. Sorrentino, S. Yamaguchi, 541–563. San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Matsumoto, D., S.H. Yoo, and S. Nakagawa. 2008. Culture, emotion regulation, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94(6):925–937.CrossRef Matsumoto, D., S.H. Yoo, and S. Nakagawa. 2008. Culture, emotion regulation, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94(6):925–937.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mayer, J.D., Y.N. Gaschke, D.L. Braverman, and T.W. Evans. 1992. Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63(1):119–132.CrossRef Mayer, J.D., Y.N. Gaschke, D.L. Braverman, and T.W. Evans. 1992. Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63(1):119–132.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Meissner, P., C. Poensgen, and T. Wulf. 2021. How hot cognition can lead us astray: the effect of anger on strategic decision making. European Management Journal 39(4):434–444.CrossRef Meissner, P., C. Poensgen, and T. Wulf. 2021. How hot cognition can lead us astray: the effect of anger on strategic decision making. European Management Journal 39(4):434–444.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mittal, V., and W.T. Ross. 1998. The impact of positive and negative affect and issue framing on issue interpretation and risk taking. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 76(3):298–324.CrossRef Mittal, V., and W.T. Ross. 1998. The impact of positive and negative affect and issue framing on issue interpretation and risk taking. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 76(3):298–324.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Miyamoto, Y., R.E. Nisbett, and T. Masuda. 2006. Culture and the physical environment: holistic versus analytic perceptual affordances. Psychological Science 17(2):113–119.CrossRef Miyamoto, Y., R.E. Nisbett, and T. Masuda. 2006. Culture and the physical environment: holistic versus analytic perceptual affordances. Psychological Science 17(2):113–119.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mok, A., and M.W. Morris. 2010. Asian-americans’ creative styles in asian and American situations: assimilative and contrastive responses as a function of bicultural identity integration. Management and Organization Review 6(03):371–390.CrossRef Mok, A., and M.W. Morris. 2010. Asian-americans’ creative styles in asian and American situations: assimilative and contrastive responses as a function of bicultural identity integration. Management and Organization Review 6(03):371–390.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Morris, M.W., and H.Y. Fu. 2001. How does culture influence conflict resolution? A dynamic constructivist analysis. Social Cognition 19(3: Special issue):324–349.CrossRef Morris, M.W., and H.Y. Fu. 2001. How does culture influence conflict resolution? A dynamic constructivist analysis. Social Cognition 19(3: Special issue):324–349.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Neumann, F., and T. Wulf. 2022. Intercultural differences in issue interpretation: effects of emotions and framing. European Management Journal 40(2):182–193.CrossRef Neumann, F., and T. Wulf. 2022. Intercultural differences in issue interpretation: effects of emotions and framing. European Management Journal 40(2):182–193.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ng, A.H., and M. Hynie. 2014. Cultural differences in indecisiveness: the role of naïve dialecticism. Personality and Individual Differences 70:45–50.CrossRef Ng, A.H., and M. Hynie. 2014. Cultural differences in indecisiveness: the role of naïve dialecticism. Personality and Individual Differences 70:45–50.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nielsen, B.B., and S. Nielsen. 2011. The role of top management team international orientation in international strategic decision-making: the choice of foreign entry mode. Journal of World Business 46(2):185–193.CrossRef Nielsen, B.B., and S. Nielsen. 2011. The role of top management team international orientation in international strategic decision-making: the choice of foreign entry mode. Journal of World Business 46(2):185–193.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nielsen, B.B., and S. Nielsen. 2013. Top management team nationality diversity and firm performance: a multilevel study. Strategic Management Journal 34(3):373–382.CrossRef Nielsen, B.B., and S. Nielsen. 2013. Top management team nationality diversity and firm performance: a multilevel study. Strategic Management Journal 34(3):373–382.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nisbett, R.E., K. Peng, I. Choi, and A. Norenzayan. 2001. Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review 108(2):291–310.CrossRef Nisbett, R.E., K. Peng, I. Choi, and A. Norenzayan. 2001. Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review 108(2):291–310.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Olson, B.J., Y. Bao, and S. Parayitam. 2007. Strategic decision making within Chinese firms: the effects of cognitive diversity and trust on decision outcomes. Journal of World Business 42(1):35–46.CrossRef Olson, B.J., Y. Bao, and S. Parayitam. 2007. Strategic decision making within Chinese firms: the effects of cognitive diversity and trust on decision outcomes. Journal of World Business 42(1):35–46.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Olson, B.J., S. Parayitam, and Y. Bao. 2016. Strategic decision making: the effects of cognitive diversity, conflict, and trust on decision outcomes. Journal of Management 33(2):196–222.CrossRef Olson, B.J., S. Parayitam, and Y. Bao. 2016. Strategic decision making: the effects of cognitive diversity, conflict, and trust on decision outcomes. Journal of Management 33(2):196–222.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Oyserman, D., and S.W.S. Lee. 2008. Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin 134(2):311–342.CrossRef Oyserman, D., and S.W.S. Lee. 2008. Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin 134(2):311–342.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pavett, C.M., and A.W. Lau. 1983. Managerial work: the influence of hierarchical level and functional specialty. Academy of Management Journal 26(1):170–177.CrossRef Pavett, C.M., and A.W. Lau. 1983. Managerial work: the influence of hierarchical level and functional specialty. Academy of Management Journal 26(1):170–177.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Payne, J.W. 1976. Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: an information search and protocol analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 16(2):366–387.CrossRef Payne, J.W. 1976. Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: an information search and protocol analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 16(2):366–387.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Peng, K., and E. Knowles. 2003. Culture, education, and the attribution of physical causality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29:1272–1284.CrossRef Peng, K., and E. Knowles. 2003. Culture, education, and the attribution of physical causality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29:1272–1284.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pettigrew, A.M., R.W. Woodman, and K.S. Cameron. 2001. Studying organizational change and development: challenges for future research. Academy of Management Journal 44(4):697–713.CrossRef Pettigrew, A.M., R.W. Woodman, and K.S. Cameron. 2001. Studying organizational change and development: challenges for future research. Academy of Management Journal 44(4):697–713.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Plourde, Y., S.C. Parker, and J.-L. Schaan. 2014. Expatriation and its effect on headquarters’ attention in the multinational enterprise. Strategic Management Journal 35(6):938–947.CrossRef Plourde, Y., S.C. Parker, and J.-L. Schaan. 2014. Expatriation and its effect on headquarters’ attention in the multinational enterprise. Strategic Management Journal 35(6):938–947.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ronen, S., and O. Shenkar. 2013. Mapping world cultures: cluster formation, sources and implications. Journal of International Business Studies 44:867–897.CrossRef Ronen, S., and O. Shenkar. 2013. Mapping world cultures: cluster formation, sources and implications. Journal of International Business Studies 44:867–897.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Russell, J.A. 1980. A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39(6):1161–1178.CrossRef Russell, J.A. 1980. A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39(6):1161–1178.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sanchez-Burks, J., and Q.N. Huy. 2009. Emotional aperture and strategic change: the accurate recognition of collective emotions. Organization Science 20(1):22–34.CrossRef Sanchez-Burks, J., and Q.N. Huy. 2009. Emotional aperture and strategic change: the accurate recognition of collective emotions. Organization Science 20(1):22–34.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Savitz, R., J. Shackman, and S.P. Fitzgerald. 2016. The impact of national culture on corporate diversification. International Journal of Business and Globalisation 17(2):243.CrossRef Savitz, R., J. Shackman, and S.P. Fitzgerald. 2016. The impact of national culture on corporate diversification. International Journal of Business and Globalisation 17(2):243.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schaefer, A., F. Nils, X. Sanchez, and P. Philippot. 2010. Assessing the effectiveness of a large database of emotion-eliciting films: a new tool for emotion researchers. Cognition & Emotion 24(7):1153–1172.CrossRef Schaefer, A., F. Nils, X. Sanchez, and P. Philippot. 2010. Assessing the effectiveness of a large database of emotion-eliciting films: a new tool for emotion researchers. Cognition & Emotion 24(7):1153–1172.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Scherer, K.R., and P. Ekman. 1984. Approaches to emotion. Hillsdale: Psychology Press. Scherer, K.R., and P. Ekman. 1984. Approaches to emotion. Hillsdale: Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwarz, N., and G.L. Clore. 1983. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of personality and social psychology 45(3):513.CrossRef Schwarz, N., and G.L. Clore. 1983. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of personality and social psychology 45(3):513.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schwenk, C.R. 1982. Why sacrifice rigour for relevance? A proposal for combining laboratory and field research in strategic management. Strategic Management Journal 3(3):213–225.CrossRef Schwenk, C.R. 1982. Why sacrifice rigour for relevance? A proposal for combining laboratory and field research in strategic management. Strategic Management Journal 3(3):213–225.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schwenk, C.R. 1995. Strategic decision making. Journal of Management 21(3):471–493.CrossRef Schwenk, C.R. 1995. Strategic decision making. Journal of Management 21(3):471–493.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Seo, M.-G., and L.F. Barrett. 2007. Being emotional during decision making: good or bad? An empirical investigation. Academy of Management Journal 50(4):923–940.CrossRef Seo, M.-G., and L.F. Barrett. 2007. Being emotional during decision making: good or bad? An empirical investigation. Academy of Management Journal 50(4):923–940.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shenkar, O. 2001. Cultural distance revisited: towards a more rigorous conceptualization and measurement of cultural differences. Journal of International Business Studies 32:519–535.CrossRef Shenkar, O. 2001. Cultural distance revisited: towards a more rigorous conceptualization and measurement of cultural differences. Journal of International Business Studies 32:519–535.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shepherd, D.A., J.S. McMullen, and W. Ocasio. 2017. Is that an opportunity? An attention model of top managers’ opportunity beliefs for strategic action. Strategic Management Journal 38(3):626–644.CrossRef Shepherd, D.A., J.S. McMullen, and W. Ocasio. 2017. Is that an opportunity? An attention model of top managers’ opportunity beliefs for strategic action. Strategic Management Journal 38(3):626–644.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Simsek, Z. 2007. CEO tenure and organizational performance: an intervening model. Strategic Management Journal 28(6):653–662.CrossRef Simsek, Z. 2007. CEO tenure and organizational performance: an intervening model. Strategic Management Journal 28(6):653–662.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Smith, C.A., and P.C. Ellsworth. 1985. Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48(4):813–838.CrossRef Smith, C.A., and P.C. Ellsworth. 1985. Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48(4):813–838.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Song, M., R.J. Calantone, and C.A. Di Benedetto. 2002. Competitive forces and strategic choice decisions: an experimental investigation in the United States and Japan. Strategic Management Journal 23(10):969–978.CrossRef Song, M., R.J. Calantone, and C.A. Di Benedetto. 2002. Competitive forces and strategic choice decisions: an experimental investigation in the United States and Japan. Strategic Management Journal 23(10):969–978.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Staw, B.M. 1976. Knee-deep in the big muddy: a study of escalating commitment to a chosen course of action. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 16(1):27–44.CrossRef Staw, B.M. 1976. Knee-deep in the big muddy: a study of escalating commitment to a chosen course of action. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 16(1):27–44.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Staw, B.M., and S.G. Barsade. 1993. Affect and managerial performance: a test of the sadder-but-wiser versus happier-and-smarter hypotheses. Administrative Science Quarterly 38:304–331.CrossRef Staw, B.M., and S.G. Barsade. 1993. Affect and managerial performance: a test of the sadder-but-wiser versus happier-and-smarter hypotheses. Administrative Science Quarterly 38:304–331.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tiedens, L.Z., and S. Linton. 2001. Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: the effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81(6):973–988.CrossRef Tiedens, L.Z., and S. Linton. 2001. Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: the effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81(6):973–988.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tosi, H.L., A.L. Brownlee, P. Silva, and J.P. Katz. 2003. An empirical exploration of decision-making under agency controls and stewardship structure. Journal of Management Studies 40(8):2053–2071.CrossRef Tosi, H.L., A.L. Brownlee, P. Silva, and J.P. Katz. 2003. An empirical exploration of decision-making under agency controls and stewardship structure. Journal of Management Studies 40(8):2053–2071.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tse, D.K., K. Lee, I. Vertinsky, and D.A. Wehrung. 1988. Does culture matter? A cross-cultural study of executives’ choice, decisiveness, and risk adjustment in international marketing. Journal of Marketing 52(4):81–95. Tse, D.K., K. Lee, I. Vertinsky, and D.A. Wehrung. 1988. Does culture matter? A cross-cultural study of executives’ choice, decisiveness, and risk adjustment in international marketing. Journal of Marketing 52(4):81–95.
Zurück zum Zitat Tsui, A.S., S.S. Nifadkar, and A.Y. Ou. 2007. Cross-national, cross-cultural organizational behavior research: advances, gaps, and recommendations. Journal of Management 33(3):426–478.CrossRef Tsui, A.S., S.S. Nifadkar, and A.Y. Ou. 2007. Cross-national, cross-cultural organizational behavior research: advances, gaps, and recommendations. Journal of Management 33(3):426–478.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tung, R.L., and A. Verbeke. 2010. Beyond Hofstede and GLOBE: improving the quality of cross-cultural research. Journal of International Business Studies 41:1259–1274.CrossRef Tung, R.L., and A. Verbeke. 2010. Beyond Hofstede and GLOBE: improving the quality of cross-cultural research. Journal of International Business Studies 41:1259–1274.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van ’t, Wout M., R.S. Kahn, A.G. Sanfey, and A. Aleman. 2006. Affective state and decision-making in the ultimatum game. Experimental Brain Research 169(4):564–568.CrossRef Van ’t, Wout M., R.S. Kahn, A.G. Sanfey, and A. Aleman. 2006. Affective state and decision-making in the ultimatum game. Experimental Brain Research 169(4):564–568.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van de Vijver, F., and K. Leung. 1997. Methods and data analysis of comparative research. Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology 1:257-300. Van de Vijver, F., and K. Leung. 1997. Methods and data analysis of comparative research. Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology 1:257-300.
Zurück zum Zitat Varnum, M.E.W., I. Grossmann, S. Kitayama, and R.E. Nisbett. 2010. The origin of cultural differences in cognition: evidence for the social orientation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19(1):9–13.CrossRef Varnum, M.E.W., I. Grossmann, S. Kitayama, and R.E. Nisbett. 2010. The origin of cultural differences in cognition: evidence for the social orientation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19(1):9–13.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wang, Q. 2008. Being American, being Asian: the bicultural self and autobiographical memory in Asian Americans. Cognition 107(2):743–751.CrossRef Wang, Q. 2008. Being American, being Asian: the bicultural self and autobiographical memory in Asian Americans. Cognition 107(2):743–751.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wiersema, M.F., and K.A. Bantel. 1992. Top management team demography and corporate strategic change. The Academy of Management Journal 35(1):91–121. Wiersema, M.F., and K.A. Bantel. 1992. Top management team demography and corporate strategic change. The Academy of Management Journal 35(1):91–121.
Zurück zum Zitat Woodside, A., R. de Villiers, and R. Marshall. 2016. Incompetency and competency training. Cham: Springer.CrossRef Woodside, A., R. de Villiers, and R. Marshall. 2016. Incompetency and competency training. Cham: Springer.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wyer, R.S., Jr, G.L. Clore, and L.M. Isbell. 1999. Affect and information processing. Advances in experimental social psychology 31:1–77.CrossRef Wyer, R.S., Jr, G.L. Clore, and L.M. Isbell. 1999. Affect and information processing. Advances in experimental social psychology 31:1–77.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Yates, J.F., and S. de Oliveira. 2016. Culture and decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136:106–118.CrossRef Yates, J.F., and S. de Oliveira. 2016. Culture and decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136:106–118.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
When We Are Happy, We Are the Same—Emotions as a Boundary Condition for the Impact of Cultural Differences on Strategic Decisions
verfasst von
Sophie Florian
Philip Meissner
Torsten Wulf
Xian Xu
Philip Yang
Publikationsdatum
10.05.2024
Verlag
Springer International Publishing
Erschienen in
Schmalenbach Journal of Business Research
Print ISSN: 0341-2687
Elektronische ISSN: 2366-6153
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s41471-024-00184-4

Premium Partner